The impact of input: language acquisition in the visually impaired

Variation in language development between blind and sighted children may result from a diminution of experience or differences in linguistic input, or it may be a product of other factors. Researchers argue about the relative weighting of these. We examine this argument by reviewing data and findings from our studies of blind children's language and we evaluate the possible impact of input, both environmental and linguistic. We show that variation cannot be uniquely attributed to either of these, but find evidence that experiential input may influence some areas while linguistic input more strongly affects others. Moreover, there is a complex interaction between these. We also find independent adaptive strategies by the children, pointing to a plasticity in the acquisition process itself.

[1]  R. Wilson The emergence of the semantics of tense and aspect in the language of a visually impaired child , 1985 .

[2]  C. Snow The development of conversation between mothers and babies , 1977, Journal of Child Language.

[3]  M. Pérez-Pereira,et al.  Pragmatic functions of blind and sighted children's language: a twin case study , 1992 .

[4]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings , 1973 .

[5]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  Mother I'd rather do it myself: the contribution of selected child listener variables' , 1977 .

[6]  Elinor Ochs,et al.  Propositions across utterances and speakers , 1979 .

[7]  Daniel N. Stern,et al.  The First Relationship: Infant and Mother , 1977 .

[8]  J. Folger,et al.  A pragmatic analysis of spontaneous imitations , 1978, Journal of Child Language.

[9]  V. Tilden,et al.  Mother and Infant , 1884, Hall's journal of health.

[10]  Lila R. Glietman Maturational determinants of language growth , 1981, Cognition.

[11]  Courtney B. Cazden,et al.  ENVIRONMENTAL ASSISTANCE TO THE CHILD'S ACQUISITION OF GRAMMAR. , 1965 .

[12]  Lois Bloom,et al.  Adult-child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  Daniel N. Stern,et al.  Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviors. , 1974 .

[14]  Anne Dunlea,et al.  Vision and the Emergence of Meaning: Blind and Sighted Children's Early Language , 1989 .

[15]  E. Andersen,et al.  Family Communication Styles and Language Development , 1984 .

[16]  Melissa Bowerman,et al.  The acquisition of word meaning: An investigation into some current conflicts , 1978 .

[17]  T. G. Nienhuys,et al.  Linguistic Feedback and Maternal Speech: Comparisons of Mothers Addressing Hearing and Hearing-impaired Children , 1980 .

[18]  L. E. Miner A Study of the Incidence of Speech Deviations among Visually Handicapped Children , 1963 .

[19]  S. Fraiberg,et al.  Insights from the Blind , 1977 .

[20]  D. Burlingham Some Notes on the Development of the Blind1 , 1961 .

[21]  J. Macnamara Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. , 1972, Psychological review.

[22]  Marilyn Shatz,et al.  On the development of communicative understandings: An early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Werner Deutsch,et al.  The Development of Verbal and Nonverbal Devices for Reference. , 1982 .

[24]  R. McKevitt Talking with children. , 1970, Journal of the New York State School Nurse-Teachers Association.

[25]  E. Andersen,et al.  Blind children's language: resolving some differences , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[26]  Patricia Marks Greenfield,et al.  Why Do Children Say What They Say When They Say It? An Experimental Approach to the Psychogenesis of Presupposition. , 1978 .

[27]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .

[28]  S. Chiat If I were you and you were me: the analysis of pronouns in a pronoun-reversing child , 1982, Journal of Child Language.

[29]  L. Rescorla,et al.  Overextension in early language development , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[30]  R. N. Goss Language used by mothers of deaf children and mothers of hearing children. , 1970, American annals of the deaf.

[31]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .

[32]  E. Klima The signs of language , 1979 .

[33]  Anne Dunlea,et al.  The emergence process: conceptual and linguistic influences on morphological development , 1992 .

[34]  Ann M. Peters,et al.  The role of Imitation in the developing syntax of a blind child , 1987 .

[35]  Ann M. Peters,et al.  What Are You Cookin' on a Hot?: Movement Constraints in the Speech of A Three-Year-Old Blind Child , 1988 .

[36]  S. Sugarman,et al.  Children's Early Thought: Developments in classification , 1983 .

[37]  T. D. Cutsforth The Unreality of Words to the Blind , 1932 .

[38]  A. Bigelow,et al.  Early words of blind children , 1987, Journal of Child Language.