Younger and older adults' schematic representations of intergenerational communication

The current paper examines younger and older adults’ cognitive representations of intergenerational conversations. In interviews, younger and older adults were asked to imagine various types of conversations with older and younger targets. They were prompted to provide a wide variety of information about the targets and the conversations. The interviews were transcribed and analyzed to uncover types of conversations commonly reported. Through a combination of coding and hierarchical cluster analysis, a hierarchical arrangement of types of conversations emerged in younger and older adults’ descriptions. Each of the types is described in detail. In a second study, exemplars of each type were sorted by younger and older adults and subjected to multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis. The results supported the validity of the types from Study 1, and suggested dimensions underlying this arrangement (positive‐negative and helping‐not helping for the younger adults; positive‐negative and high‐low change orientation for the older adults). The findings are discussed in terms of the communication predicament of aging model, and the role that these representations of conversations may play in future research. It is argued that knowledge of these cognitive representations of communication provides a new perspective on the ways in which intergenerational interactions may progress.

[1]  C. Stangor,et al.  Stereotypes and Stereotyping , 1996 .

[2]  Stereotyping of older adults: the role of target facial cues and perceiver characteristics. , 1997 .

[3]  J. Harwood,et al.  Expectations for Communication with Positive and Negative Subtypes of Older Adults , 1998, International journal of aging & human development.

[4]  H. Giles,et al.  Psycholinguistic and social psychological components of communication by and with the elderly , 1986 .

[5]  H. Giles,et al.  Patronizing the Young: Forms and Evaluations , 1994, International journal of aging & human development.

[6]  J. Harwood Young adults' cognitive representations of intergenerational conversations , 1998 .

[7]  C. Berger,et al.  Social Cognition and Communication , 1982 .

[8]  B. G. Rule,et al.  Anatomy of a persuasion schema: Targets, goals, and strategies. , 1985 .

[9]  B. O'Connor,et al.  Perceptions of baby talk, frequency of receiving baby talk, and self-esteem among community and nursing home residents. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[10]  H. Giles,et al.  Accommodating the elderly: Invoking and extending a theory , 1988, Language in Society.

[11]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  C. Carver,et al.  Schema‐Guided Information Search in Stereotyping of the Elderly1 , 1984 .

[13]  Klaus Krippendorff,et al.  Content Analysis: An Introduction to Its Methodology , 1980 .

[14]  M. L. Hummert,et al.  Stereotypes of the elderly and patronizing speech. , 1994 .

[15]  W. Gekoski,et al.  Contact with and perceptions of the elderly. , 1986, The Gerontologist.

[16]  M. Snyder When belief creates reality , 1984 .

[17]  R. Wyer Associated systems theory : a systematic approach to cognitive representations of persons , 1994 .

[18]  M. Snyder,et al.  Thinking about ourselves and others: Self-monitoring and social knowledge , 1980 .

[19]  Mark P. Zanna,et al.  The Nonverbal Mediation of Self-Fulfilling Prophecies in Interracial Interaction. , 1974 .

[20]  M. L. Hummert,et al.  Stereotyping of older adults: the role of target facial cues and perceiver characteristics. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[21]  H. Giles,et al.  Older adults' trait ratings of three age-groups around the Pacific rim , 2001, Journal of cross-cultural gerontology.

[22]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  SCRIPTS, PLANS, GOALS, AND UNDERSTANDING , 1988 .

[23]  M. L. Hummert,et al.  Multiple stereotypes of elderly and young adults: a comparison of structure and evaluations. , 1990, Psychology and aging.

[24]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Memory and Cognition in Its Social Context , 1989 .

[25]  E B Ryan,et al.  Changing the Way We Talk with Elders: Promoting Health Using the Communication Enhancement Model , 1995, International journal of aging & human development.

[26]  H. Giles,et al.  Young People's Beliefs About Intergenerational Communication , 1997 .

[27]  Ellen Bouchard Ryan,et al.  Communication Predicaments of Aging , 1995 .

[28]  H. Giles,et al.  'Your're Only As Old As You Sound': Perceived Vocal Age and Social Meanings , 1996 .

[29]  Howard Giles,et al.  Intergenerational Conversations Young Adults' Retrospective Accounts , 1996 .

[30]  J. Harwood,et al.  Communicative Predictors of Solidarity in the Grandparent-Grandchild Relationship , 2000 .

[31]  S H Ng,et al.  College students' trait ratings of three age groups around the Pacific Rim , 1996, Journal of cross-cultural gerontology.

[32]  M. L. Hummert,et al.  Communication With Older Adults The Influence of Age Stereotypes, Context, and Communicator Age , 1998 .

[33]  H. Giles,et al.  Reactions to Older People being Patronized , 1996 .

[34]  Kathy Kellermann The Conversation MOP II. Progression Through Scenes in Discourse , 1991 .

[35]  L. Caporael The paralanguage of caregiving: baby talk to the institutionalized aged. , 1981, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. , 1977 .

[37]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Contact and Conflict in Intergroup Encounters , 1986 .