A memory system in the monkey.

A neural model is presented, based largely on evidence from studies in monkeys, postulating that coded representation of stimuli are stored in the higher-order sensory (i.e. association) areas of the cortex whenever stimulus activation of these areas also triggers a cortico-limbo-thalamo-cortical circuit. This circuit, which could act as either an imprinting or rehearsal mechanism, may actually consist of two parallel circuits, one involving the amygdala and the dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus, and the other the hippocampus and the anterior nuclei. The stimulus representation stored in cortex by action of these circuits is seen as mediating three different memory processes: recognition, which occurs when the stored representation is reactivated via the original sensory pathway; recall, when it is reactivated via any other pathway; and association, when it activates other stored representations (sensory, affective, spatial, motor) via the outputs of the higher-order sensory areas to the relevant structures.

[1]  G. Bonin,et al.  The neocortex of Macaca mulatta , 1947 .

[2]  O MARBURG,et al.  The amygdaloid complex. , 1949, Confinia neurologica.

[3]  M. Mishkin Visual discrimination performance following partial ablations of the temporal lobe. II. Ventral surface vs. hippocampus. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  B. Milner MEMORY AND THE MEDIAL TEMPORAL REGIONS OF THE BRAIN , 1970 .

[5]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus--reinforcement associations. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[6]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Cortical Visual Areas and Their Interactions , 1972 .

[7]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Recognition impaired and association intact in the memory of monkeys after transection of the fornix. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[8]  M Mishkin,et al.  An analysis of short-term visual memory in the monkey. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  Deepak N. Pandya,et al.  Some connections of the entorhinal (area 28) and perirhinal (area 35) cortices of the rhesus monkey. III. Efferent connections , 1975, Brain Research.

[10]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual activation of neurons in inferotemporal cortex depends on striate cortex and forebrain commissures. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  D. Perl,et al.  Diencephalic amnesia: a reappraisal. , 1976, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[12]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Pattern discrimination thresholds after partial inferior temporal or lateral striate lesions in monkeys , 1977, Brain Research.

[13]  M. Mishkin Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not by separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus , 1978, Nature.

[14]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Analogous neural models for tactual and visual learning , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  M Mishkin,et al.  Organization of the amygdalopetal projections from modality‐specific cortical association areas in the monkey , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[16]  Feinberg Jf The Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. , 1980 .

[17]  M. Herkenham Laminar organization of thalamic projections to the rat neocortex. , 1980, Science.

[18]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Evidence for the sequential participation of inferior temporal cortex and amygdala in the acquisition of stimulus-reward associations , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  L. Squire,et al.  Retrograde amnesia and remote memory impairment , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[21]  E. Iwai,et al.  Further evidence on elevated discrimination limens for reduced patterns in monkeys with inferotemporal lesions , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Visual recognition impairment following medial thalamic lesions in monkeys , 1983, Neuropsychologia.