Fearful faces heighten the cortical representation of contextual threat

Perception of facial expressions is typically investigated by presenting isolated face stimuli. In everyday life, however, faces are rarely seen without a surrounding visual context that affects perception and interpretation of the facial expression. Conversely, fearful faces may act as a cue, heightening the sensitivity of the visual system to effectively detect potential threat in the environment. In the present study, we used steady-state visually evoked potentials (ssVEPs) to examine the mutual effects of facial expressions (fearful, neutral, happy) and affective visual context (pleasant, neutral, threat). By assigning two different flicker frequencies (12 vs. 15Hz) to the face and the visual context scene, cortical activity to the concurrent stimuli was separated, which represents a novel approach to independently tracking the cortical processes associated with the face and the context. Twenty healthy students viewed flickering faces overlaid on flickering visual scenes, while performing a simple change-detection task at fixation, and high-density EEG was recorded. Arousing background scenes generally drove larger ssVEP amplitudes than neutral scenes. Importantly, background and expression interacted: When viewing fearful facial expressions, the ssVEP in response to threat context was amplified compared to other backgrounds. Together, these findings suggest that fearful faces elicit vigilance for potential threat in the visual periphery.

[1]  J. Gray,et al.  Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[2]  Matthias M Müller,et al.  Attentional Bias to Briefly Presented Emotional Distractors Follows a Slow Time Course in Visual Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  R. Zeelenberg,et al.  Emotion-induced trade-offs in spatiotemporal vision. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Competition effects of threatening faces in social anxiety. , 2012, Emotion.

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  M. Trimble,et al.  The Human Amygdala , 2010 .

[7]  Roman Feiman,et al.  Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Tobias Brosch,et al.  Faces in Context: A Review and Systematization of Contextual Influences on Affective Face Processing , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[9]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Social vision: Sustained perceptual enhancement of affective facial cues in social anxiety , 2011, NeuroImage.

[11]  Shlomo Bentin,et al.  The automaticity of emotional face-context integration. , 2011, Emotion.

[12]  J D Victor,et al.  Fluctuations of steady-state VEPs: interaction of driven evoked potentials and the EEG. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[13]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Face-Evoked Steady-State Visual Potentials: Effects of Presentation Rate and Face Inversion , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Andreas Keil,et al.  The neural correlates of feature-based selective attention when viewing spatially and temporally overlapping images , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Andreas Keil,et al.  The Timing of Emotional Discrimination in Human Amygdala and Ventral Visual Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  B. Rossion,et al.  Robust sensitivity to facial identity in the right human occipito-temporal cortex as revealed by steady-state visual-evoked potentials. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[17]  S. Maxson,et al.  Animal Models of Human Behavior , 2006 .

[18]  Søren K. Andersen,et al.  Time course of affective bias in visual attention: Convergent evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials and behavioral data , 2010, NeuroImage.

[19]  René Zeelenberg,et al.  Emotional cues enhance the attentional effects on spatial and temporal resolution , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  Maital Neta,et al.  Valence resolution of ambiguous facial expressions using an emotional oddball task. , 2011, Emotion.

[21]  Beatrice de Gelder,et al.  Context influences early perceptual analysis of faces--an electrophysiological study. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  B. de Gelder,et al.  Rapid influence of emotional scenes on encoding of facial expressions: an ERP study. , 2008, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[23]  Markus Junghöfer,et al.  ElectroMagnetoEncephalography Software: Overview and Integration with Other EEG/MEG Toolboxes , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[24]  Skyler T. Hawk,et al.  Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database , 2010 .

[25]  Christian Büchel,et al.  Emotional Processing in a Salient Motion Context: Integration of Motion and Emotion in Both V5/hMT+ and the Amygdala , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Brian N. Pasley,et al.  Subcortical Discrimination of Unperceived Objects during Binocular Rivalry , 2004, Neuron.

[27]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  An objective method for measuring face detection thresholds using the sweep steady-state visual evoked response. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[28]  Thomas Armstrong,et al.  Emotional expressions and visual search efficiency: specificity and effects of anxiety symptoms. , 2011, Emotion.

[29]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[30]  P. Lang,et al.  Re‐entrant projections modulate visual cortex in affective perception: Evidence from Granger causality analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[31]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Time course of competition for visual processing resources between emotional pictures and foreground task. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  Cue-Invariant Networks for Figure and Background Processing in Human Visual Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Context Is Routinely Encoded During Emotion Perception , 2010, Psychological science.

[34]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[35]  Peng Zhang,et al.  Binocular Rivalry Requires Visual Attention , 2011, Neuron.

[36]  R. Zeelenberg,et al.  Emotion Improves and Impairs Early Vision , 2009, Psychological science.

[37]  R. Zeelenberg,et al.  Dissociating emotion-induced blindness and hypervision. , 2009, Emotion.

[38]  Changjun Shi,et al.  Visual Pathways Involved in Fear Conditioning Measured with Fear-Potentiated Startle: Behavioral and Anatomic Studies , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  C. Darwin The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , .

[40]  M. Carrasco,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Emotion Facilitates Perception and Potentiates the Perceptual Benefits of Attention , 2022 .

[41]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady‐state visual evoked potential , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[42]  Vincent D Costa,et al.  Tagging cortical networks in emotion: A topographical analysis , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[43]  D. Amaral,et al.  Topographic organization of projections from the amygdala to the visual cortex in the macaque monkey , 2003, Neuroscience.

[44]  Mark W Becker Panic Search , 2009, Psychological science.

[45]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Early modulation of visual perception by emotional arousal: Evidence from steady-state visual evoked brain potentials , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  J. M. Carroll,et al.  Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  M. Bradley,et al.  Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. , 1994, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[48]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  Angry, Disgusted, or Afraid? , 2008, Psychological science.

[49]  Paul J. Whalen,et al.  Amygdaloid contributions to conditioned arousal and sensory information processing. , 1992 .

[50]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Perceiving Threat In the Face of Safety: Excitation and Inhibition of Conditioned Fear in Human Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic recording of steadystate visual evoked cortical activity , 2005, Brain Topography.

[52]  R. Kleck,et al.  Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. , 2005, Emotion.

[53]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Contributions of the Amygdala to Emotion Processing: From Animal Models to Human Behavior , 2005, Neuron.

[54]  P. Lang,et al.  Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[55]  J. Vroomen,et al.  The perception of emotions by ear and by eye , 2000 .

[56]  E. Lauterbach The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction , 1993 .

[57]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  A steady-state visual evoked potential approach to individual face perception: Effect of inversion, contrast-reversal and temporal dynamics , 2012, NeuroImage.

[58]  R. Righart,et al.  Recognition of facial expressions is influenced by emotional scene gist , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[59]  R. Dolan,et al.  Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  J. R. Hughes,et al.  Human brain electrophysiology. Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine , 1989 .

[61]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[62]  Scott M. Hayes,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Context Effects on Face Recognition: Automatic Binding and Context Shift Decrements , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  D. Regan Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine , 1989 .

[64]  David R. Herring,et al.  Electrophysiological responses to evaluative priming: the LPP is sensitive to incongruity. , 2011, Emotion.

[65]  S. Andersen,et al.  Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[66]  P. Lang International affective picture system (IAPS) : affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual , 2005 .

[67]  P. Whalen Fear, Vigilance, and Ambiguity , 1998 .

[68]  Derek K. Jones,et al.  Occipito-temporal connections in the human brain. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[69]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Sustained Preferential Processing of Social Threat Cues: Bias without Competition? , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  Antonio Schettino,et al.  rain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention : hat is magic and what is not , 2012 .

[71]  Lamme Vaf,et al.  Why visual attention and awareness are different , 2003 .

[72]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[73]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Additive effects of emotional content and spatial selective attention on electrocortical facilitation. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[74]  B. Kapp,et al.  Effects of electrical stimulation of the amygdaloid central nucleus on neocortical arousal in the rabbit. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[75]  A. Keil,et al.  Temporal Trade-Off Effects in Sustained Attention: Dynamics in Visual Cortex Predict the Target Detection Performance during Distraction , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.