The Andean anacrusis? Rhythmic structure and perception in Easter songs of Northern Potosí, Bolivia

This paper is the result of a collaboration between an ethnomusicologist (Henry Stobart) and music psychologist (Ian Cross). It examines the interaction of a variety of processes underlying the rhythmic structure and perception of a song genre of the Bolivian Andes: these include linguistic prosody, movement patterns, perceptual constraints and the dynamics of the culture's musical aesthetics. The “Easter songs “ which form the focus of this study, present particular problems of rhythmic perception for outsiders to the culture (such as the authors), who often tend to misperceive these songs as anacrustic. This phenomenon is addressed through an exploration of the unequal proportions and accent placement in the charango accompaniment, and an analysis of stress patterns of Quechua (and Aymara), the languages in which these songs are sung. It is shown that the first syllable of a phrase is treated as a functional “downbeat” and, despite outsiders’ perceptions, the anacrusis appears to be absent from the Quechua and Aymara musical genres of the region. The paper questions whether these findings might be relevant to other musical genres of the Andes, and considers the problems of perception in the transcription and analysis of Andean music.

[1]  Robin Higgins,et al.  How Musical is Man , 1974 .

[2]  Christopher S. Lee The perception of metrical structure: Experimental evidence and a new model , 1987 .

[3]  John Blacking,et al.  Venda Children's Songs: A Study in Ethnomusicological Analysis , 1995 .

[4]  Kofi Agawu The Invention of African Rhythm , 1995 .

[5]  R. Jackendoff Consciousness and the Computational Mind , 1987 .

[6]  Bruno Nettl The study of ethnomusicology : twenty-nine issues and concepts , 1985 .

[7]  John Blacking How Musical Is Man , 1973 .

[8]  Julien Tiersot,et al.  La musique des Incas et ses survivances , 1926 .

[9]  T. Turino The Charango and the "Sirena": Music, Magic, and the Power of Love , 1983 .

[10]  Paul Proulx Quechua and Aymara , 1987 .

[11]  B. Isbell,et al.  The Ontogenesis of Metaphor: Riddle Games among Quechua Speakers Seen as Cognitive Discovery Procedures , 1977 .

[12]  John A. Sloboda,et al.  The performance of music , 1986 .

[13]  Marilyn Strathern,et al.  Out of Context: The Persuasive Fictions of Anthropology [and Comments and Reply] , 1987, Current Anthropology.

[14]  Marilyn Strathern,et al.  Out of Context: The Persuasive Fictions of Anthropology , 1990 .

[15]  Bruce Mannheim Popular song and popular grammar, poetry and metalanguage , 1986 .

[16]  Yoshitaka Nakajima,et al.  A New Illusion of Time Perception , 1991 .

[17]  M. d'Harcourt,et al.  La musique des Incas et ses survivances , 1926 .

[18]  Jacquetta Hill Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning , 1998 .

[19]  S. Harnad Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition , 1990 .

[20]  Jeff Pressing,et al.  Cognitive multiplicity in polyrhythmic pattern performance. , 1996 .

[21]  Charles Seeger,et al.  PRESCRIPTIVE AND DESCRIPTIVE MUSIC-WRITING , 1958 .

[22]  Gerhard Kubik Pattern Perception and Recognition in African Music , 1979 .

[23]  Briony Jane Williams Stress in modern Welsh , 1983 .

[24]  D. Hargreaves The development of artistic and musical competence , 1996 .

[25]  M. Jones,et al.  Attending to auditory events: The role of temporal organization. , 1993 .

[26]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. , 1976, Psychological review.

[27]  C. Myers A STUDY OF RHYTHM IN PRIMITIVE MUSIC1 , 1905 .

[28]  R. Jackendoff,et al.  A Generative Theory of Tonal Music , 1985 .