Multisensory Integration and Internal Models for Sensing Gravity Effects in Primates

Gravity is crucial for spatial perception, postural equilibrium, and movement generation. The vestibular apparatus is the main sensory system involved in monitoring gravity. Hair cells in the vestibular maculae respond to gravitoinertial forces, but they cannot distinguish between linear accelerations and changes of head orientation relative to gravity. The brain deals with this sensory ambiguity (which can cause some lethal airplane accidents) by combining several cues with the otolith signals: angular velocity signals provided by the semicircular canals, proprioceptive signals from muscles and tendons, visceral signals related to gravity, and visual signals. In particular, vision provides both static and dynamic signals about body orientation relative to the vertical, but it poorly discriminates arbitrary accelerations of moving objects. However, we are able to visually detect the specific acceleration of gravity since early infancy. This ability depends on the fact that gravity effects are stored in brain regions which integrate visual, vestibular, and neck proprioceptive signals and combine this information with an internal model of gravity effects.

[1]  Bernd Fritzsch,et al.  Development and evolution of the vestibular sensory apparatus of the mammalian ear. , 2005, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[2]  D. Angelaki,et al.  Vestibular system: the many facets of a multimodal sense. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  An integrative neural network for detecting inertial motion and head orientation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Christian Darlot,et al.  Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movements , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.

[5]  Ronald J. White,et al.  Humans in space , 2001, Nature.

[6]  B. Hood Gravity does rule for falling events , 1998 .

[7]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Contributions of the Human Temporoparietal Junction and MT/V5+ to the Timing of Interception Revealed by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  L. Snyder,et al.  A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception , 2009, Neuron.

[9]  Jessika Weiss,et al.  Vision Science Photons To Phenomenology , 2016 .

[10]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Representation of Visual Gravitational Motion in the Human Vestibular Cortex , 2005, Science.

[11]  Guy Orban,et al.  Processing of targets in smooth or apparent motion along the vertical in the human brain: an fMRI study. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  W P Medendorp,et al.  Fusion of visual and vestibular tilt cues in the perception of visual vertical. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  John B. Pittenger,et al.  Detection of Violations of the Law of Pendulum Motion: Observers' Sensitivity to the Relation Between Period and Length , 1990 .

[14]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  E S Spelke,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to effects of gravity on visible object motion. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Alessandro Moscatelli,et al.  Implied Dynamics Biases the Visual Perception of Velocity , 2014, PloS one.

[17]  D M Merfeld,et al.  Humans use internal models to estimate gravity and linear acceleration , 1999, Nature.

[18]  H. Mittelstaedt A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical , 1983, Naturwissenschaften.

[19]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[20]  P. Werkhoven,et al.  Visual processing of optic acceleration , 1992, Vision Research.

[21]  Kathleen Cullen,et al.  The Vestibular System , 2003 .

[22]  Jean Laurens,et al.  Bayesian processing of vestibular information , 2007, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  A Bayesian model of the disambiguation of gravitoinertial force by visual cues , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  B J Hess,et al.  Computation of Inertial Motion: Neural Strategies to Resolve Ambiguous Otolith Information , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Lauren Scharff,et al.  Spatial disorientation: decades of pilot fatalities. , 2011, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[26]  L. Zupan,et al.  Neural processing of gravitoinertial cues in humans. III. Modeling tilt and translation responses. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Vincenzo Maffei,et al.  Simulated Self-motion in a Visual Gravity Field: Sensitivity to Vertical and Horizontal , 2022 .

[28]  W. Precht,et al.  Vestibular mechanisms. , 1979, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  Dora E. Angelaki,et al.  Neurons compute internal models of the physical laws of motion , 2004, Nature.

[30]  Lionel Carmant,et al.  Revisiting the role of the insula in refractory partial epilepsy , 2009, Epilepsia.

[31]  Yury Petrov,et al.  Locus of spatial attention determines inward-outward anisotropy in crowding. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[32]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III. Response dynamics. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  S Glasauer Interaction of Semicircular Canals and Otoliths in the Processing Structure of the Subjective Zenith , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[34]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  When Up Is Down in 0g: How Gravity Sensing Affects the Timing of Interceptive Actions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Cerebral representations for egocentric space: functional-anatomical evidence from caloric vestibular stimulation and neck vibration , 2001, NeuroImage.

[37]  R. Mayne,et al.  A Systems Concept of the Vestibular Organs , 1974 .

[38]  D. Angelaki,et al.  The functional significance of velocity storage and its dependence on gravity , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  D. Angelaki,et al.  Neural Representation of Orientation Relative to Gravity in the Macaque Cerebellum , 2013, Neuron.

[40]  T. Brandt,et al.  The Vestibular Cortex: Its Locations, Functions, and Disorders , 1999, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[41]  Vincenzo Maffei,et al.  Vestibular nuclei and cerebellum put visual gravitational motion in context. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  M. Grealy,et al.  Judging Time Intervals Using a Model of Perceptuo-Motor Control , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to static tilts and to long-duration centrifugal force. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  The weight of time: gravitational force enhances discrimination of visual motion duration. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[46]  T. Brandt,et al.  Multisensory cortical signal increases and decreases during vestibular galvanic stimulation (fMRI). , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  R. Fitzpatrick,et al.  The vestibular system , 2005, Current Biology.

[48]  Vincenzo Maffei,et al.  Anticipating the effects of visual gravity during simulated self-motion: estimates of time-to-passage along vertical and horizontal paths , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.