Perceived Heaviness Is Influenced by the Style of Lifting

This experiment examined the influence of action on weight perception and the size-weight illusion. Participants rated the perceived heaviness of objects that varied in mass, length, and width. Half of the participants lifted each object and placed it down on the table and half placed the object on a pedestal before reporting their perception of heaviness. These tasks were performed either with or without vision. In all cases, increases in size produced decreases in perceived heaviness. For increases in both length and width, the use of vision produced a greater decrease in perceived heaviness. For increases in width alone, the task in which participants placed the object on a pedestal (a task for which the width of the object was a relevant variable) was associated with a greater decrease in perceived heaviness. Salience of information was discussed as a means by which task and modality might influence perception.

[1]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  The Role of Rotational Inertia in the Haptic and Haptic + Visual Size-Weight Illusions , 2003 .

[2]  Paul Koseleff Studies in the Perception of Heaviness. II , 1958 .

[3]  Crystal D. Oberle,et al.  Independence and separability of volume and mass in the size-weight illusion , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.

[5]  A. Garg,et al.  Maximum acceptable weights and maximum voluntary isometric strengths for asymmetric lifting. , 1986, Ergonomics.

[6]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Théodore Flournoy,et al.  Illusions de poids , 1894 .

[8]  B. O'Shaughnessy The sense of touch , 1989 .

[9]  M. Turvey,et al.  Eigenvalues of the inertia tensor and exteroception by the “muscular sense” , 1994, Neuroscience.

[10]  A Mital,et al.  Psychophysical and physiological responses to lifting symmetrical and asymmetrical loads symmetrically and asymmetrically. , 1986, Ergonomics.

[11]  S. Lederman,et al.  The role of haptic versus visual volume cues in the size-weight illusion , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  M T Turvey,et al.  Attunement, Calibration, and Exploration in Fast Haptic Perceptual Learning , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  H E Ross,et al.  Weight Illusions and Weight Discrimination—a Revised Hypothesis , 1970, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Eric L. Amazeen,et al.  The Effects of volume on Perceived Heaviness by Dynamic Touch: With and Without Vision , 1997 .

[15]  F. B. Dresslar Studies in the psychology of touch , 1894 .

[16]  S H Snook,et al.  A Study of Size, Distance, Height, and Frequency Effects on Manual Handling Tasks , 1983, Human factors.

[17]  E L Amazeen,et al.  Perceptual independence of size and weight by dynamic touch. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Arun Garg,et al.  Container Characteristics and Maximum Acceptable Weight of Lift1 , 1980 .

[19]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[20]  Anil Mital,et al.  Development of Strength and Capacity Norms for Manual Materials Handling Activities: The State of the Art , 1980 .

[21]  M T Turvey,et al.  Weight perception and the haptic size-weight illusion are functions of the inertia tensor. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  K. Shockley,et al.  Rotational kinematics influence multimodal perception of heaviness , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  W. Smith The Integrative Action of the Nervous System , 1907, Nature.

[24]  Eric L Amazeen,et al.  Sensory and perceptual interactions in weight perception , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Paul Koseleff,et al.  Studies in the perception of heaviness. I.1.2: Some relevant facts concerning the size-weight-effect (SWE) , 1957 .

[27]  Kevin Shockley,et al.  Rotational inertia and multimodal heaviness perception , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Mechanical invariants are implicated in dynamic touch as a function of their salience in the stimulus flow. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Perceiving Affordances for Aperture Crossing for the Person-Plus-Object System , 2005 .

[30]  C. M. Davis,et al.  Lifting movements in the size-weight illusion , 1976 .

[31]  M. Turvey,et al.  Affordance, proper function, and the physical basis of perceived heaviness , 1999, Cognition.

[32]  Kevin Shockley,et al.  Metamers in the haptic perception of heaviness and moveableness , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  A. Cowey,et al.  Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  E. Amazeen,et al.  Perceiving heaviness by dynamic touch: An investigation of the size-weight illusion in preschoolers , 2002 .

[36]  Judy Czylok A SENSE OF TOUCH , 1979, Nursing standard (Royal College of Nursing (Great Britain) : 1987).

[37]  H. C. Warren De l'influence de la perception visuelle des corps sur leur poids apparent. , 1896 .

[38]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[39]  T Landis,et al.  Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[40]  W. Epstein,et al.  Perceiving Distance: A Role of Effort and Intent , 2004, Perception.

[41]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[42]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[43]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size–weight illusion , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  I. Kingma,et al.  The Inertia Tensor Versus Static Moment and Mass in Perceiving Length and Heaviness of Hand-Wielded Rods , 2002 .

[46]  W. Epstein,et al.  Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Charles Bell,et al.  On the Nervous Circle Which Connects the Voluntary Muscles with the Brain , 1826, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

[48]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  Rich Gossweiler,et al.  Perceiving geographical slant , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.