Phonological activation of category coordinates during speech planning is observable in children but not in adults: evidence for cascaded processing.

There is a long-standing debate in the area of speech production on the question of whether only words selected for articulation are phonologically activated (as maintained by serial-discrete models) or whether this is also true for their semantic competitors (as maintained by forward-cascading and interactive models). Past research has addressed this issue by testing whether retrieval of a target word (e.g., cat) affects--or is affected by--the processing of a word that is phonologically related to a semantic category coordinate of the target (e.g., doll, related to dog) and has consistently failed to obtain such mediated effects in adult speakers. The authors present a series of experiments demonstrating that mediated effects are present in children (around age 7) and diminish with increasing age. This observation provides further evidence for cascaded models of lexical retrieval.

[1]  M. Garrett Processes in language production , 1988 .

[2]  David P. Vinson,et al.  Is "Count" and "Mass" Information Available When the Noun Is Not? An Investigation of Tip of the Tongue States and Anomia , 1999 .

[3]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Information flow in the mental lexicon during speech planning: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[4]  G. Dell,et al.  Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.

[5]  A. Paivio,et al.  Cognitive components of picture naming. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  Frederick J. Newmeyer Language : psychological and biological aspects , 1988 .

[7]  Nadine Martin,et al.  Phonological Facilitation of Semantic Errors in Normal and Aphasic Speakers , 1996 .

[8]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  N. Martin,et al.  Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: Implications for models of lexical retrieval☆ , 1989 .

[10]  W. Glaser,et al.  The time course of picture-word interference. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[12]  Richard R. Rosinski,et al.  Picture-word interference is semantically based. , 1977 .

[13]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[14]  Donald Shankweiler,et al.  Phonological processes in literacy : a tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman , 1991 .

[15]  C J Johnson,et al.  Cognitive components of naming in children: effects of referential uncertainty and stimulus realism. , 1992, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  J. Perkell,et al.  Invariance and variability in speech processes , 1987 .

[17]  A. Caramazza How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .

[18]  Padraig G. O'Seaghdha,et al.  Mediated Semantic-Phonological Priming: Calling Distant Relatives , 1997 .

[19]  A. Walley,et al.  The gating paradigm: Effects of presentation format on spoken word recognition by children and adults , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  L. Menn,et al.  Phonological development : models, research, implications , 1994 .

[21]  Amanda C. Walley,et al.  The Role of Vocabulary Development in Children′s Spoken Word Recognition and Segmentation Ability , 1993 .

[22]  Victor S Ferreira,et al.  Phonological Influences on Lexical (Mis)Selection , 2003, Psychological science.

[23]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Context effects on lexical choice and lexical activation. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  W. Marslen-Wilson Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition , 1987, Cognition.

[25]  Michele Miozzo,et al.  Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  Stephen J. Lupker,et al.  The semantic nature of response competition in the picture-word interference task , 1979 .

[27]  David Friedman,et al.  Developmental picture norms: Relationships between name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity for child and adult ratings of two sets of line drawings , 1989 .

[28]  Geoffrey Underwood,et al.  SEMANTIC INTERFERENCE FROM UNATTENDED PRINTED WORDS , 1976 .

[29]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. , 1995 .

[30]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .

[31]  A. Guilford,et al.  Standardization of the Boston Naming Test at the kindergarten and elementary school levels. , 1988 .

[32]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Semantic and phonological influences on picture naming by children and teenagers , 2002 .

[33]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Semantic and phonological activation in noun and pronoun production. , 2001, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.

[34]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .

[36]  W. Levelt Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations , 1992, Cognition.

[37]  A. Walley Spoken word recognition by young children and adults , 1988 .

[38]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging , 1992 .

[39]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Discrete serial versus cascaded processing in lexical access in speech production : Further evidence from the coactivation of near-synonyms , 1998 .

[40]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. , 1975 .

[41]  R. R. Peterson,et al.  Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .

[42]  J. Metsala,et al.  Age-of-Acquisition, Word Frequency, and Neighborhood Density Effects on Spoken Word Recognition by Children and Adults , 2001 .

[43]  G. Dell,et al.  Stages of lexical access in language production , 1992, Cognition.

[44]  S. Tipper,et al.  Mechanisms of attention: a developmental study. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[45]  Peter A. Starreveld,et al.  On the Interpretation of Onsets of Auditory Context Effects in Word Production , 2000 .

[46]  G. Dell,et al.  Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: a comment on Levelt et al. (1991). , 1991, Psychological review.

[47]  M. Garrett Disorders of lexical selection , 1992, Cognition.

[48]  F. Newmeyer Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey , 1989 .

[49]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data , 1981 .

[50]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. , 1990 .

[51]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Do Words Really Interfere in Naming Pictures , 1976 .

[52]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  M. Denckla,et al.  Rapid "automatized" naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. , 1974, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.