Non-native phonemes in adult word learning: evidence from the N400m
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Saffran. Constraints on Statistical Language Learning , 2002 .
[2] Jane Garry,et al. Facts about the world's languages : an encyclopedia of the world's major languages, past and present , 2001 .
[3] M. Rugg. The effects of semantic priming and work repetition on event-related potentials. , 1985, Psychophysiology.
[4] E. Halgren,et al. Human medial temporal lobe potentials evoked in memory and language tasks. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[5] M. Turvey,et al. The motor theory of speech perception reviewed , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] A. Nakamura,et al. Localizing the distributed language network responsible for the N400 measured by MEG during auditory sentence processing , 2006, Brain Research.
[7] M. Goldsmith,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .
[8] P. Kuhl. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[9] K Lehnertz,et al. Inferior temporal stream for word processing with integrated mnemonic function , 2001, Human brain mapping.
[10] Klaus Lehnertz,et al. Human temporal lobe potentials in verbal learning and memory processes , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[11] G. McCarthy,et al. Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic priming , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[12] Patricia K. Kuhl,et al. Language/Culture/Mind/Brain , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[13] M. Junghöfer,et al. Early Left-Hemispheric Dysfunction of Face Processing in Congenital Prosopagnosia: An MEG Study , 2008, PloS one.
[14] Colin M. Brown,et al. Lexical-semantic event-related potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions and aphasia, and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[15] Matthias J. Sjerps,et al. The bounds on flexibility in speech perception. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] P. D. Eimas,et al. Distinctive Feature Codes in the Short-Term Memory of Children. , 1975 .
[17] K. Stevens,et al. Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.
[18] Pienie Zwitserlood,et al. Plasticity of the human auditory cortex induced by discrimination learning of non-native, mora-timed contrasts of the Japanese language. , 2002, Learning & memory.
[19] B. N'Kaoua,et al. Differential involvement of the human temporal lobe structures in short- and long-term memory processes assessed by intracranial ERPs. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[20] P. Kuhl. Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[21] A. Liberman,et al. Some effects of later-occurring information on the perception of stop consonant and semivowel , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] Heikki Lyytinen,et al. Cortical Activation during Spoken-Word Segmentation in Nonreading-Impaired and Dyslexic Adults , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] T. Allison,et al. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.
[24] S. Kuriki,et al. MEG study on neural activities associated with syntactic and semantic violations in spoken Korean sentences , 2005, Neuroscience Research.
[25] C. Petten,et al. Conceptual relationships between spoken words and environmental sounds: Event-related brain potential measures , 1995, Neuropsychologia.
[26] P. Holcomb,et al. Event-Related Brain Potentials Reflect Semantic Priming in an Object Decision Task , 1994, Brain and Cognition.
[27] M. Kutas,et al. A preliminary comparison of the N400 response to semantic anomalies during reading, listening and signing. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[28] Colin M. Brown,et al. Spoken Sentence Comprehension in Aphasia: Event-related Potential Evidence for a Lexical Integration Deficit , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] H. Neville,et al. Language and , 2019, Adventure Diffusion.
[30] J Hillenbrand,et al. Speech perception by infants: categorization based on nasal consonant place of articulation. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[31] Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells,et al. First- and Second-language Phonological Representations in the Mental Lexicon , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] A. Liberman,et al. The motor theory of speech perception revised , 1985, Cognition.
[33] A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al. Watching the brain during meaning acquisition. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[34] Arthur M. Jacobs,et al. Word, Pseudoword, and Nonword Processing: A Multitask Comparison Using Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[35] L. Osterhout,et al. Neural correlates of second-language word learning: minimal instruction produces rapid change , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[36] P. Kuhl. Human adults and human infants show a “perceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] C. Petten,et al. Neural localization of semantic context effects in electromagnetic and hemodynamic studies , 2006, Brain and Language.
[38] Pienie Zwitserlood,et al. Five days versus a lifetime: intense associative vocabulary training generates lexically integrated words. , 2007, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.
[39] J. Werker,et al. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life , 1984 .
[40] 吉川 清隆,et al. Perception of Speech , 1970 .
[41] Caterina Breitenstein,et al. Development and validation of a language learning model for behavioral and functional-imaging studies , 2002, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[42] R. Lasky,et al. VOT discrimination by four to six and a half month old infants from Spanish environments. , 1975, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[43] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates , 2006, Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing.
[44] Andrew C Papanicolaou,et al. Source localization of the N400 response in a sentence-reading paradigm using evoked magnetic fields and magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Brain Research.
[45] M. Kutas,et al. The Search for Common Sense: An Electrophysiological Study of the Comprehension of Words and Pictures in Reading , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] J. Kissler,et al. Buzzwords , 2007, Psychological science.
[47] R. Näätänen,et al. Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[48] P. D. Eimas,et al. Speech Perception in Infants , 1971, Science.
[49] Ruth de Diego Balaguer,et al. First- and second-language phonological representations in the mental lexicon , 2006 .
[50] A M Liberman,et al. Perception of the speech code. , 1967, Psychological review.
[51] Thomas Wolbers,et al. Hippocampus activity differentiates good from poor learners of a novel lexicon , 2005, NeuroImage.
[52] P D Eimas,et al. Contextual effects in infant speech perception. , 1980, Science.
[53] Janet F. Werker,et al. Cross-Language Speech Perception : Initial Capabilities and Developmental Change , 2001 .
[54] M. Kutas,et al. Influences of semantic and syntactic context on open- and closed-class words , 1991, Memory & cognition.
[55] P K Kuhl,et al. Perceptual strategies in prelingual speech segmentation , 1993, Journal of Child Language.
[56] Stefan Knecht,et al. New Names for Known Things: On the Association of Novel Word Forms with Existing Semantic Information , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[57] A. Friederici,et al. First-Pass versus Second-Pass Parsing Processes in a Wernicke's and a Broca's Aphasic: Electrophysiological Evidence for a Double Dissociation , 1998, Brain and Language.
[58] G. Bernardi,et al. Intracellular responses of caudate neurons to brain stem stimulation. , 1970, Brain research.
[60] C. Petten. A comparison of lexical and sentence-level context effects in event-related potentials , 1993 .
[61] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[62] F. Pulvermüller,et al. Language outside the focus of attention: The mismatch negativity as a tool for studying higher cognitive processes , 2006, Progress in Neurobiology.
[63] Markus Junghöfer,et al. Selective Visual Attention to Emotion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[64] E. Ringelstein,et al. Levodopa: Faster and better word learning in normal humans , 2004, Annals of neurology.
[65] P. Kuhl,et al. Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. , 2005, Developmental science.
[66] D. Pisoni,et al. Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[68] Ava J. Senkfor,et al. Electrophysiological dissociation between verbal and nonverbal semantic processing in learning disabled adults , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[69] P. Strevens. Spectra of Fricative Noise in Human Speech , 1960 .
[70] M. Kutas,et al. Interactions between sentence context and word frequencyinevent-related brainpotentials , 1990, Memory & cognition.