Scientific Utopia: I. Opening Scientific Communication
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Arif E. Jinha. Article 50 million: an estimate of the number of scholarly articles in existence , 2010, Learn. Publ..
[2] A. Greenwald,et al. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[3] P. Seglen,et al. Education and debate , 1999, The Ethics of Public Health.
[4] Wolfgang Viechtbauer,et al. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments , 2007, Psychometrika.
[5] Steven J. Bell,et al. The blended librarian: A blueprint for redefining the teaching and learning role of academic librarians , 2004 .
[6] A. Greenwald. Consequences of Prejudice Against the Null Hypothesis , 1975 .
[7] David R. Jones,et al. Empirical assessment of effect of publication bias on meta-analyses , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.
[8] Leandre R. Fabrigar,et al. The Review Process at PSPB: Correlates of Interreviewer Agreement and Manuscript Acceptance , 1999 .
[9] Jelte M. Wicherts,et al. Psychology must learn a lesson from fraud case , 2011, Nature.
[10] David Berle,et al. Inconsistencies between reported test statistics and p‐values in two psychiatry journals , 2007, International journal of methods in psychiatric research.
[11] F. Godlee,et al. Effect of open peer review on quality of reviews and on reviewers'recommendations: a randomised trial , 1999, BMJ.
[12] Daniele Fanelli,et al. Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries , 2011, Scientometrics.
[13] S Goldbeck-Wood,et al. Evidence on peer review—scientific quality control or smokescreen? , 1999, BMJ.
[14] J. Glynn,et al. Open access policy , 2013, The Lancet.
[15] Brian D. Cameron,et al. Trends in the Usage of ISI Bibliometric Data: Uses, Abuses, and Implications , 2005 .
[16] Stevan Harnad,et al. The invisible hand of peer review , 1998 .
[17] Vincent Larivière,et al. Self-Selected or Mandated, Open Access Increases Citation Impact for Higher Quality Research , 2010, PloS one.
[18] J. Schooler. Unpublished results hide the decline effect , 2011, Nature.
[19] Samuel Ball,et al. The Peer Review Process Used to Evaluate Manuscripts Submitted to Academic Journals: Interjudgmental Reliability , 1989 .
[20] D. V. Essen,et al. Cognitive neuroscience 2.0: building a cumulative science of human brain function , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Denny Borsboom,et al. Letting the daylight in: Reviewing the reviewers and other ways to maximize transparency in science , 2012, Front. Comput. Neurosci..
[22] David Green,et al. An Open Access Overview , 2012 .
[23] Charles Oppenheim,et al. The citation advantage of open-access articles , 2008, J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol..
[24] Bo-Christer Björk,et al. Scientific journal publishing: yearly volume and open access availability , 2009, Inf. Res..
[25] Pamela W. Smith. The National Institutes of Health (NIH) , 2008 .
[26] M Sievert,et al. Phenomena of retraction: reasons for retraction and citations to the publications. , 1998, JAMA.
[27] M. Borenstein,et al. Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments , 2006 .
[28] William H. Starbuck,et al. How Much Better are the Most Prestigious Journals? The Statistics of Academic Publication , 2005, Organ. Sci..
[29] G. Smith,et al. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test , 1997, BMJ.
[30] P. Tetlock,et al. Accounting for the effects of accountability. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.
[31] D. Fanelli. “Positive” Results Increase Down the Hierarchy of the Sciences , 2010, PloS one.
[32] Steven L. Miller,et al. Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[33] J. Ioannidis. Why Most Published Research Findings Are False , 2005, PLoS medicine.
[34] L. Bornmann,et al. A Reliability-Generalization Study of Journal Peer Reviews: A Multilevel Meta-Analysis of Inter-Rater Reliability and Its Determinants , 2010, PloS one.
[35] D. W. Fiske,et al. But the Reviewers Are Making Different Criticisms of My Paper! Diversity and Uniqueness in Reviewer Comments. , 1990 .
[36] Jim Giles,et al. PR's 'pit bull' takes on open access , 2007, Nature.
[37] C. Sunstein. Republic.com 2.0 , 2007 .
[38] J. Armstrong,et al. Peer review for journals: Evidence on quality control, fairness, and innovation , 1997 .
[39] Robert Rosenthal,et al. How often are our numbers wrong , 1978 .
[40] D F Horrobin,et al. The philosophical basis of peer review and the suppression of innovation. , 1990, JAMA.
[41] A. Greenwald,et al. Under what conditions does theory obstruct research progress? , 1986, Psychological review.
[42] R Smith,et al. Opening up BMJ peer review , 1999, BMJ.
[43] D. Benos,et al. The ups and downs of peer review. , 2007, Advances in physiology education.
[44] Joseph S. Rossi,et al. How Often are Our Statistics Wrong? a Statistics Class Exercise , 1987 .
[45] G. Eysenbach. The Open Access Advantage , 2006, Journal of medical Internet research.
[46] W. K. Simmons,et al. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.
[47] Burt V. Bronk,et al. Hierarchy of sciences , 1977 .
[48] S. Harnad,et al. Open access to peer-reviewed research through author/institution self-archiving: maximizing research impact by maximizing online access. , 2003, Journal of postgraduate medicine.
[49] Michael Jubb,et al. Open Access - What Are the Economic Benefits? A Comparison of the United Kingdom, Netherlands and Denmark , 2010, Learn. Publ..
[50] Per Ottar Seglen,et al. Causal relationship between article citedness and journal impact , 1994 .
[51] J. Ioannidis. Why Most Published Research Findings Are False , 2005 .
[52] Timothy D. Wilson,et al. Scientists' Evaluations of Research: the Biasing Effects of the Importance of the Topic , 1993 .
[53] Jindřiška Svobodová,et al. How Science Works , 2014 .
[54] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[55] E. García‐Berthou,et al. Incongruence between test statistics and P values in medical papers , 2004 .
[56] Grover J. Whitehurst,et al. Interrater agreement for journal manuscript reviews. , 1984 .
[57] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Implicit social cognition: from measures to mechanisms , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[58] Philip E. Tetlock,et al. Biases in research evaluation: Inflated assessment, oversight, or error-type weighting? , 2007 .
[59] Patrick Onghena,et al. An Assessment of the Predictive Validity of Impact Factor Scores: Implications for Academic Employment Decisions in Social Work , 2006 .
[60] J. Houghton,et al. Economic implications of alternative scholarly publishing models : exploring the costs and benefits. JISC EI-ASPM Project. A report to the Joint Information Systems Committee (JISC) , 2009 .
[61] H. Yarandi,et al. Empirical developments in retraction , 2008, Journal of Medical Ethics.
[62] T. Sterling. Publication Decisions and their Possible Effects on Inferences Drawn from Tests of Significance—or Vice Versa , 1959 .
[63] J. Wicherts,et al. The (mis)reporting of statistical results in psychology journals , 2011, Behavior research methods.
[64] Emma Marris. American Chemical Society: Chemical reaction , 2005, Nature.
[65] William Y. Arms. What are the alternatives to peer review? Quality Control in Scholarly Publishing on the Web , 2002 .
[66] R. Rosenthal. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results , 1979 .
[67] J. Scott. Peer Review for Journals: Evidence on Quality Control, Fairness, and Innovation , 1997 .
[68] V. Stodden. Trust your science? Open your data and code , 2011 .
[69] David Moher,et al. Comparison of registered and published primary outcomes in randomized controlled trials. , 2009, JAMA.
[70] David J. Hardisty,et al. Diffusion of treatment research: does Open Access matter? , 2008, Journal of clinical psychology.
[71] N. Adler,et al. When Knowledge Wins: Transcending the Sense and Nonsense of Academic Rankings , 2009 .
[72] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Scientific Utopia , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[73] John P A Ioannidis,et al. Excess significance bias in the literature on brain volume abnormalities. , 2011, Archives of general psychiatry.
[74] R. Merton,et al. Patterns of evaluation in science: Institutionalisation, structure and functions of the referee system , 1971 .
[75] Fred Lewis Muhl. A public library , 1904 .
[76] Jennifer M. Urban,et al. Shining Light into Black Boxes , 2012, Science.
[77] John D. Norton,et al. How science works , 1998 .
[78] Matthew B. Jones,et al. Challenges and Opportunities of Open Data in Ecology , 2011, Science.
[79] Philip M. Davis,et al. Open access publishing, article downloads, and citations: randomised controlled trial , 2008, BMJ : British Medical Journal.
[80] D. Cicchetti. The reliability of peer review for manuscript and grant submissions: A cross-disciplinary investigation , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[81] D. Borsboom,et al. The poor availability of psychological research data for reanalysis. , 2006, The American psychologist.
[82] Lowell L. Hargens,et al. Scholarly Consensus and Journal Rejection Rates. , 1988 .
[83] S. Ceci,et al. Peer-review practices of psychological journals: The fate of published articles, submitted again , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[84] P. Sleight,et al. Publication bias , 1991, The Lancet.
[85] Theodor D. Sterling,et al. Publication decisions revisited: the effect of the outcome of statistical tests on the decision to p , 1995 .
[86] K. Antelman. Do Open-Access Articles Have a Greater Research Impact? , 2004 .