Pharmacological modulation of prefrontal cortical activity during a working memory task in young and older humans: a PET study with physostigmine.

OBJECTIVE Age-associated cholinergic dysfunction may contribute to the cognitive decline observed during aging, including a decline in working memory. The current study was designed to determine how healthy aging influences the neural response to working memory before and during pharmacological potentiation of the cholinergic system. METHOD In 13 young and 13 older healthy volunteers, regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was measured by using [15O]H2O and positron emission tomography across 10 scans that alternated between a working-memory-for-faces task and rest. In all subjects, the first two scans were obtained during intravenous saline infusion. Seven young and eight older subjects then received intravenous infusion of physostigmine, a cholinesterase inhibitor, and the remaining six young and five older subjects continued to receive saline. RESULTS In the placebo condition, task-specific rCBF increases in prefrontal regions were observed in the right middle and inferior frontal cortices in young subjects and in more anterior and ventral prefrontal regions in older individuals. Physostigmine during the working memory task significantly improved performance in both age groups. The right prefrontal regions that were selectively recruited in each age group during the placebo condition showed significantly lower rCBF during physostigmine administration. CONCLUSIONS Cholinergic enhancement does not affect structurally defined cortical regions but rather modulates neural activity in functionally defined regions, that is, in task-related prefrontal cortical areas that are selectively and distinctively recruited in young and older subjects.

[1]  T. Robbins,et al.  Central cholinergic systems and cognition. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[2]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Memory and cognitive function in the elderly. A preliminary trial of physostigmine. , 1980, Archives of neurology.

[4]  P Pietrini,et al.  Preferential metabolic involvement of visual cortical areas in a subtype of Alzheimer's disease: clinical implications. , 1996, The American journal of psychiatry.

[5]  S. Sikström,et al.  Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  J. Buccafusco,et al.  The Cholinergic Hypothesis of Age and Alzheimer's Disease-Related Cognitive Deficits: Recent Challenges and Their Implications for Novel Drug Development , 2003, Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics.

[7]  R. Bartus,et al.  The cholinergic hypothesis of geriatric memory dysfunction. , 1982, Science.

[8]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Aging Gracefully: Compensatory Brain Activity in High-Performing Older Adults , 2002, NeuroImage.

[9]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Age-related reductions in human recognition memory due to impaired encoding. , 1995, Science.

[10]  D. Jenden,et al.  Brain acetylcholine synthesis declines with senescence. , 1981, Science.

[11]  Cheryl L Grady,et al.  Changes in memory processing with age , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  G. Alexander,et al.  The neurometabolic landscape of cognitive decline: in vivo studies with positron emission tomography in Alzheimer's disease. , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  C. Geula,et al.  Cortical cholinergic fibers in aging and Alzheimer's disease: A morphometric study , 1989, Neuroscience.

[14]  J E Desmond,et al.  Age differences in prefrontal cortical activity in working memory. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[15]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Effect of task difficulty on cerebral blood flow during perceptual matching of faces , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[16]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a Human Experimental Psychology Dementia and Working Memory Dementia and Working Memory , 2022 .

[17]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The decline of working memory in Alzheimer's disease. A longitudinal study. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  Hemispheric differences in neural systems for face working memory: A PET‐rCBF study , 1995 .

[19]  J. Haxby,et al.  Cholinergic stimulation alters performance and task-specific regional cerebral blood flow during working memory. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Drucker-Colín,et al.  Nicotine, brain nicotinic receptors, and neuropsychiatric disorders. , 2000, Archives of medical research.

[22]  P Pietrini,et al.  Time Course of Pharmacodynamic and Pharmacokinetic Effects of Physostigmine Assessed by Functional Brain Imaging in Humans , 2000, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[23]  S. Wisniewski,et al.  Upregulation of choline acetyltransferase activity in hippocampus and frontal cortex of elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment , 2002, Annals of neurology.

[24]  S. Rapoport,et al.  The aging human brain , 1985, Annals of neurology.

[25]  G. Alexander,et al.  Cholinergic enhancement improves performance on working memory by modulating the functional activity in distinct brain regions: a positron emission tomography regional cerebral blood flow study in healthy humans , 2000, Brain Research Bulletin.

[26]  K. Davis,et al.  Cholinergic markers in elderly patients with early signs of Alzheimer disease. , 1999, JAMA.

[27]  D. Drachman,et al.  Human memory and the cholinergic system. A relationship to aging? , 1974, Archives of neurology.

[28]  R. Cabeza Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[29]  R. Bartus,et al.  On Neurodegenerative Diseases, Models, and Treatment Strategies: Lessons Learned and Lessons Forgotten a Generation Following the Cholinergic Hypothesis , 2000, Experimental Neurology.

[30]  J. Haxby,et al.  Cholinergic enhancement and increased selectivity of perceptual processing during working memory. , 2000, Science.

[31]  A. Bentivoglio,et al.  Aging is associated with a diffuse impairment of forebrain cholinergic neurons , 1990, Brain Research.

[32]  K. Oduro,et al.  Glycopyrrolate methobromide 2. Comparison with atropine sulphate in anaesthesia , 1975, Canadian Anaesthetists' Society journal.

[33]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[37]  Anthony R. McIntosh,et al.  Age-Related Changes in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow during Working Memory for Faces , 1998, NeuroImage.

[38]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human prefrontal cortex activation during a spatial working memory task. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  M. Mesulam The cholinergic lesion of Alzheimer's disease: pivotal factor or side show? , 2004, Learning & memory.