Acquisition of spatial knowledge during navigation: the role of internal and external factors

The general aim of this project of research was to investigate the role of internal and external factors involved in the construction of a spatial representation during navigation. The main question of this dissertation was: do we need the same type of memory to retrace a route and find a shortcut? In other words: which type of memory is involved in the construction of route and survey representation respectively? In research the main question is about the role of working memory in the construction of spatial representation. According to Baddeley's model (1986), working memory is not a unitary system, but it is possible to distinguish an attentional control system—the central executive—and two subsystems—the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad, which encodes and maintains verbal information and visuospatial information, respectively (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). Until now, research carried out to date on spatial representation has looked at the role of verbal (VWM) and visuo-spatial (VSWM) working memory in the construction of route representation during navigation (Garden et al., 2002; Meilinguer et al., 2008). However it remains unclear the involvement of working memory in the construction of survey knowledge. In addition, the construction of survey representation, as recent frameworks suggested (Montello et al., 1999; Kitchin et al., 1994), seems to depend also on individual difference, but little is known about the role of external factors such as the presence of landmarks or the influence of specific instructions in guiding navigation behaviour. A series of four experiments was carried out. In all experiments we investigated the role of both subcomponents of working memory through classic paradigm of dual task. Participants learned a route in a virtual (Experiments 1, 3 and 4,) or real environment (Experiment 2) performing spatial or verbal secondary task simultaneously. Reproduction of the route, pointing task, drawing map and finding a shortcut were used to investigate the construction of route and survey representation during navigation. Our results supported that the ability to retrace a route depends on encoding and maintaining the information in VWM and VSW whereas the ability to find a shortcut seems to be related on the involvement of VSWM (Experiment 1 – 2). In addition, our results confirmed that there are large individual differences in both ability to learn spatial layout and in how spatial layout is preferentially encoded (Experiment 2), and added that the sense of direction becomes the predictor of the acquisition of survey knowledge in the learning conditions without landmarks (Experiment 3). Moreover our results showed that the process of acquisition of spatial knowledge implicates also external factors, demonstrating, specifically, that the presence of landmarks and receiving specific instructions about the task facilitate the construction of mental representation (Experiment 4). In conclusion our results add to the growing body of literature supporting that the acquisition of spatial knowledge is a multi-level process influenced by internal and external factors.

[1]  Carryl L. Baldwin,et al.  Individual Differences in Route-Learning Strategy and Associated Working Memory Resources , 2009, Hum. Factors.

[2]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning , 2006 .

[3]  Gender, Graphicacy and Geography , 1986 .

[4]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. , 2002 .

[5]  L. Hasher,et al.  Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .

[6]  R. Downs,et al.  Image and Environment: Cognitive Mapping and Spatial Behavior , 2017 .

[7]  Sharin E. Garden,et al.  Visuo-spatial working memory in navigation , 2002 .

[8]  J. L. Gould,et al.  The Insect Mind: Physics or Metaphysics? , 1982 .

[9]  M. Heil,et al.  Wayfinding behavior and spatial knowledge of adults and children in a virtual environment : The role of the environmental structure , 2007 .

[10]  Andrew P. Duchon,et al.  Do Humans Integrate Routes Into a Cognitive Map? Map- Versus Landmark-Based Navigation of Novel Shortcuts , 2005 .

[11]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[12]  Derek A. Hamilton,et al.  Virtual navigation in humans: the impact of age, sex, and hormones on place learning , 2005, Hormones and Behavior.

[13]  F. Pazzaglia,et al.  Orientamento e rappresentazione dello spazio , 2004 .

[14]  E. Coluccia,et al.  The relationship between map drawing and spatial orientation abilities: A study of gender differences , 2007 .

[15]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[16]  Xiaoqian J Chai,et al.  Sex differences in directional cue use in a virtual landscape. , 2009, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  Raffaella Nori,et al.  Cognitive Styles: Errors in Directional Judgments , 2003, Perception.

[18]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[19]  Tommy Gärling,et al.  Memory for the spatial layout of the everyday physical environment: Factors affecting rate of acquisition , 1981 .

[20]  C. Lawton,et al.  Gender Differences in Wayfinding Strategies and Anxiety About Wayfinding: A Cross-Cultural Comparison , 2002 .

[21]  G L Allen,et al.  The acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity , 1988, Psychological research.

[22]  C. C. Trowbridge ON FUNDAMENTAL METHODS OF ORIENTATION AND "IMAGINARY MAPS". , 1913, Science.

[23]  F. Dyer,et al.  Honey bee spatial memory: use of route-based memories after displacement , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  D. Holding,et al.  Acquisition of Route Network Knowledge by Males and Females , 1989 .

[26]  T. Collett,et al.  Animal Navigation: Path Integration, Visual Landmarks and Cognitive Maps , 2004, Current Biology.

[27]  H. Taylor,et al.  Goal-specific influences on the representation of spatial perspective , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[28]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Wayfinding behavior and spatial knowledge of adults and children in a virtual environment: The role of landmarks. , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[29]  R. Golledge Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes , 2010 .

[30]  Toru Ishikawa,et al.  Individual Differences in the Encoding Processes of Egocentric and Allocentric Survey Knowledge , 2013, Cogn. Sci..

[31]  Tad T. Brunyé,et al.  When goals constrain: Eye movements and memory for goal‐Oriented map study , 2009 .

[32]  Sven C. Mueller,et al.  Gender differences in spatial navigation in virtual space: implications when using virtual environments in instruction and assessment , 2006, Virtual Reality.

[33]  R. Logie Visuo-spatial Working Memory , 1994 .

[34]  Edward Chace Tolman,et al.  "Insight" in rats , 1930 .

[35]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  Barbara Rogoff,et al.  Influence of the goal on children's exploration and memory of large-scale space , 1986 .

[37]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Integrating Knowledge of Vertically Aligned Large-Scale Spaces , 1993 .

[38]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatial knowledge in a young blind child , 1984, Cognition.

[39]  A. Baddeley Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Toru Ishikawa,et al.  Working memory in spatial knowledge acquisition: Differences in encoding processes and sense of direction , 2011 .

[41]  M. Sholl The relation between sense of direction and mental geographic updating , 1988 .

[42]  Andrew P. Duchon,et al.  Humans do not switch between path knowledge and landmarks when learning a new environment , 2007, Psychological research.

[43]  D. Saucier,et al.  Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[44]  C. Lawton STRATEGIES FOR INDOOR WAYFINDING: THE ROLE OF ORIENTATION , 1996 .

[45]  Michael A. Motes,et al.  Individual differences in the representations of novel environments , 2005 .

[46]  Philip M. Corsi Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. , 1972 .

[47]  Mark Blades,et al.  Developmental differences in the ability to give route directions from a map , 1992 .

[48]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  The effect of landmarks in human path integration. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[49]  Earl Hunt,et al.  Spatial Representations of Virtual Mazes: The Role of Visual Fidelity and Individual Differences , 2001, Hum. Factors.

[50]  D. R. Montello A New Framework for Understanding the Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge in Large-Scale Environments , 1998 .

[51]  Miles C. Bowman,et al.  Sex differences in the effect of articulatory or spatial dual-task interference during navigation , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[52]  C. Lawton Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety , 1994 .

[53]  E. Coluccia,et al.  Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review , 2004 .

[54]  C. Zimring,et al.  Just Down The Road A Piece , 2003 .

[55]  M. H. Matthews Gender, Home Range and Environmental Cognition , 1987 .

[56]  E. Coluccia,et al.  Learning from maps: the role of visuo‐spatial working memory , 2008 .

[57]  Yong Jeong,et al.  Spatial cognition , 2004, Neurology.

[58]  R. Long,et al.  Predicting environmental learning from spatial abilities: An indirect route , 1996 .

[59]  Jerry Weisman,et al.  Evaluating Architectural Legibility , 1981 .

[60]  R. Cohen,et al.  The Representation of Landmarks and Routes. , 1980 .

[61]  G. Evans,et al.  Environmental Learning and Cognitive Mapping , 1981 .

[62]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[63]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Working Memory in Wayfinding - A Dual Task Experiment in a Virtual City , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[64]  Gabriele Janzen,et al.  A neural wayfinding mechanism adjusts for ambiguous landmark information , 2010, NeuroImage.

[65]  Francesca Pazzaglia,et al.  Perspective, Instruction, and Cognitive Style in Spatial Representation of a Virtual Environment , 2007, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[66]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[67]  Romedi Passini,et al.  Wayfinding in Architecture , 1984 .

[68]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Spatial representation and behavior across the life span , 1981 .

[69]  Mark Blades,et al.  Wayfinding Theory and Research: The Need for a New Approach , 1991 .

[70]  Christoph Hölscher,et al.  Taxonomy of Human Wayfinding Tasks: A Knowledge-Based Approach , 2009, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[71]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 1974 .

[72]  Gary L. Allen,et al.  The impact of a wayfinder's goal on learning a new environment: Different types of spatial knowledge as goals , 1995 .

[73]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[74]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  Differenze individuali nella rappresentazione dello spazio e nell'abilità di orientamento: presentazione di un questionario autovalutativo , 2000 .

[75]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[76]  Bruce K. Britton,et al.  Effects of the organization of text on memory: Texts of two implications of a selective attention hypothesis. , 1979 .

[77]  Andrea Bosco,et al.  Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies , 2004, Applied cognitive psychology.

[78]  R. De Beni,et al.  Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals , 2001 .

[79]  R. Downs,et al.  Cognitive Maps and Spatial Behaviour: Process and Products , 2011 .

[80]  T. Gärling,et al.  Acquisition of locational information about reference points during locomotion with and without a concurrent task: effects of number of reference points. , 1981, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[81]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  What working memory subcomponents are needed in the acquisition of survey knowledge? Evidences from direction estimation and shortcut tasks , 2014, CogSci.

[82]  M. Denis,et al.  Spatial discourse and navigation: an analysis of route directions in the city of Venice , 1999 .

[83]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[84]  D. Wechsler Manual of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised , 1974 .

[85]  Sarah S. Chance,et al.  Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .

[86]  B. Roskos-Ewoldsen,et al.  THE RELATIONS AMONG WAYFINDING STRATEGY USE, SENSE OF DIRECTION, SEX, FAMILIARITY, AND WAYFINDING ABILITY , 2000 .

[87]  M. Sholl,et al.  The relation of sex and sense of direction to spatial orientation in an unfamiliar environment , 2000 .

[88]  Kay M. Stanney,et al.  Virtual environments , 2002 .

[89]  R. Kitchin,et al.  lncreasing the integrity of cognitive mapping research: appraising conceptual schemata of environment-behaviour interaction , 1996 .

[90]  Wilfried Brauer,et al.  Spatial Cognition III , 2003, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[91]  D. Saucier,et al.  Relationship among Environmental Pointing Accuracy, Mental Rotation, Sex, and Hormones , 2004 .

[92]  N. Schmajuk Cognitive maps , 1998 .

[93]  Markus Knauff,et al.  Up the down staircase : Wayfinding strategies in multi-level buildings , 2006 .

[94]  E. Lindberg,et al.  Cognitive Mapping of Large-Scale Environments , 1984 .

[95]  J. G. Quinn,et al.  Interference in Visual Working Memory , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[96]  Andrea Bosco,et al.  The role of visuo-spatial working memory in map learning: new findings from a map drawing paradigm , 2007, Psychological research.

[97]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[98]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Navigating Buildings in "Desk-Top" Virtual Environments: Experimental Investigations Using Extended Navigational Experience , 1997 .

[99]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[100]  P. Halmos I have a photographic memory , 1987 .

[101]  Max J. Egenhofer,et al.  Spatial and temporal reasoning in geographic information systems , 1998 .

[102]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Spatial Memory of Real Environments, Virtual Environments, and Maps , 2004 .

[103]  L. Kozlowski,et al.  Sense of Direction, Spatial Orientation, and Cognitive Maps. , 1977 .