Foraging in a complex naturalistic environment: capacity of spatial working memory in flower bats

SUMMARY Memory systems have evolved under selection pressures, such as the need to remember the locations of resources or past events within spatiotemporally dynamic natural environments. The full repertoire of complex behaviours exhibited by animals in dynamic surroundings are, however, difficult to elicit within simply structured laboratory environments. We have developed a computer-controlled naturalistic environment with 64 feeders for simulating dynamic patterns of water or food resource availability (depletion and replenishment) within the laboratory. The combination of feeder and cage remote control permits the automated transfer of animals between cage and test arena and, therefore, high experimental throughput and minimal disturbance to the animals (bats and mice). In the present study, we investigated spatial working memory in nectar-feeding bats (Glossophaga soricina, Phyllostomidae) collecting food from a 64-feeder array. Feeders gave only single rewards within trials so that efficient foraging required bats to avoid depleted locations. Initially, bats tended to revisit feeders (win-stay), but within three trials changed towards a win-shift strategy. The significant avoidance of revisits could not be explained by algorithmic search guiding movement through the array nor by scent cues left by the bats themselves and, thus, the data suggest that bats remembered spatial locations depleted of food. An examination of the recency effect on spatial working memory after bats shifted to a win-shift strategy indicated that bats held more than 40 behaviour actions (feeder visits) in working memory without indication of decay. This result surpasses previous findings for other taxa.

[1]  Automated Monitoring Using Acoustical Sensors for Insects in Farm-Stored Wheat , 1996 .

[2]  M. Appleby,et al.  Perception of time duration by domestic hens , 2002 .

[3]  D. von Helversen,et al.  Object recognition by echolocation: a nectar-feeding bat exploiting the flowers of a rain forest vine , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[4]  D. Olton,et al.  Animal Behavior Processes , 2022 .

[5]  C. L. Gass,et al.  SHOULD FORAGERS REMEMBER WHERE THEY'VE BEEN? EXPLORATIONS OF A SIMULATION MODEL BASED ON THE BEHAVIOR AND ENERGETICS OF TERRITORIAL HUMMINGBIRDS , 1987 .

[6]  York Winter,et al.  Hierarchical strategy for relocating food targets in flower bats: spatial memory versus cue-directed search , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  G. S. Brown Spatial association learning by rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus): effects of relative spacing among stimuli. , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[8]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[9]  G. Dehnhardt,et al.  Mental rotation in a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.

[10]  Thomas H. Kunz,et al.  Ecology of Bats , 1984, Springer US.

[11]  Jean-Arcady Meyer,et al.  BIOLOGICALLY BASED ARTIFICIAL NAVIGATION SYSTEMS: REVIEW AND PROSPECTS , 1997, Progress in Neurobiology.

[12]  H. G. Baker,et al.  On the calculation of sugar concentration in flower nectar , 1979, Oecologia.

[13]  Y. Winter,et al.  Cognitive Ecology of Pollination: Bats as pollinators: foraging energetics and floral adaptations , 2001 .

[14]  A. Gerhardt,et al.  New Online Biomonitoring System for Gammarus pulex (L.) (Crustacea): In Situ Test Below a Copper Effluent in South Sweden , 1998 .

[15]  C. L. Gass Experimental studies of foraging in complex laboratory environments , 1978 .

[16]  J. Andrade-Cetto Object Recognition , 2003 .

[17]  Glenn D. Sutherland,et al.  Learning and remembering of spatial patterns by hummingbirds , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  A. Kamil,et al.  Spatial Learning as an Adaptation in Hummingbirds , 1982, Science.

[19]  A. Redish Beyond the Cognitive Map: From Place Cells to Episodic Memory , 1999 .

[20]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .

[21]  Paul E. Hamburger,et al.  On an automated method , 1966, ACM '66.

[22]  H. G. Baker,et al.  THE PREDICTIVE VALUE OF NECTAR CHEMISTRY TO THE RECOGNITION OF POLLINATOR TYPES , 1990 .

[23]  J. Krebs,et al.  How marsh tits find their hoards: the roles of site preference and spatial memory. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[24]  C. Janson Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  Kathryn J. Jeffery,et al.  The neurobiology of spatial behaviour , 2003 .

[26]  Heinz Stephan,et al.  Comparative Neurobiology in Chiroptera , 1996 .

[27]  Clifton Lee Gass A digital encoder for field recording of behavioral, temporal, and spatial information in directly computer-accessible form , 1977 .

[28]  D S Olton,et al.  Spatial memory. , 1977, Scientific American.

[29]  Y. C. Li,et al.  A modularized infrared light matrix system with high resolution for measuring animal behaviors , 1993, Physiology & Behavior.

[30]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Computations on metric maps in mammals: getting oriented and choosing a multi-destination route. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[31]  Gwo-Jen Jan,et al.  A new automated method for detection and recording of animal moving path , 1995, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[32]  D. Robert,et al.  Tracking of flying insects using pan-tilt cameras , 2000, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[33]  William A. Roberts,et al.  Spatial memory in the rat on a hierarchical maze , 1979 .

[34]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .