The effect of selective spatial attention on peripheral discrimination thresholds

Experiments were conducted to investigate the role of attention in peripheral detection and discrimination. Advance spatial cues informed subjects about likely target positions; the task required to detect/discriminate plus localise a target briefly presented at cued or uncued locations, with accuracy as the dependent variable ("cost-benefit" analysis).Spatial cueing produced reliable advantages for cued over uncued locations, in single and in multiple element displays. However, costs plus benefits were less marked for single displays. Thus, advance knowledge of the likely target location enhances performance also when there are no competing stimuli present in the visual field. But costs plus benefits are smaller because single target onsets at uncued locations summon attention in the same "automatic" fashion as peripheral cues. Peripheral cues trigger a rapid facilitatory component (automatic), fading out within 300 msec after cue onset. Facilitation is then maintained by a less effective mechanism (controlled). Central cues initiate only this second component. Sustained, controlled, orienting in response to central cues is interruptable by automatic orienting in response to uninformative peripheral flashes. Interruption also occurs when irrelevant flashes compete with peripheral cues. However, interference is less marked for the early automatic than for the following controlled orienting component. Indication of a second position (four-location display) to be most likely resulted in a marked sensitivity gain for this position, relative to uncued locations in a single cue condition. That is, attention could be simultaneously shared between two cued positions. For a luminance detection task (single target), cued locations showed no advantage in sensitivity; but for letter detection tasks (target plus distractors), there was a marked priming effect. That is, letter detection is capacity limited, whereas luminance detection is not. In all tasks, decision criteria are largely preset according to a-priori target probabilities assigned to particular locations, i.e. more liberal for cued and more conservative for uncued locations.

[1]  A H van der Heijden,et al.  The Effect of Cueing in a Visual Signal Detection Task , 1973, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[3]  Martin Shepherd,et al.  EMDISP: A visual display system with digital and analogue sampling , 1984 .

[4]  Y Tsal,et al.  On interpreting the effects of location preknowledge: A critique of Duncan , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Stephen J. Boies,et al.  Components of attention. , 1971 .

[6]  H. Pashler,et al.  Visual attention and stimulus identification. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  R J Watt,et al.  Image segmentation at contour intersections in human focal vision. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  B. Julesz,et al.  Human factors and behavioral science: Textons, the fundamental elements in preattentive vision and perception of textures , 1983, The Bell System Technical Journal.

[9]  U. Neisser Cognitive Psychology: Classic Edition , 1967 .

[10]  L. Stone Theory of Optimal Search , 1975 .

[11]  R. A. Kinchla,et al.  The role of structural redundancy in the perception of visual targets , 1977 .

[12]  J. A. Kleiss,et al.  Locus and persistence of capacity limitations in visual information processing. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  H. Woodrow The measurement of attention , 1914 .

[14]  F. L. Engel Visual conspicuity, directed attention and retinal locus. , 1971, Vision research.

[15]  Bruno Breitmeyer,et al.  The role of on and off transients in determining the psychophysical spatial frequency response , 1975, Vision Research.

[16]  R. M. Warren,et al.  HELMHOLTZ ON PERCEPTION: ITS PHYSIOLOGY AND DEVELOPMENT. , 1970 .

[17]  John Duncan,et al.  The demonstration of capacity limitation , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  T. Cohn,et al.  Detectability of a luminance increment: effect of spatial uncertainty. , 1974, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[19]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Valerie Townsend,et al.  Voluntary Attention in Peripheral Vision and its Effects on Acuity and Differential Thresholds , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  D. M. Green,et al.  Speed-accuracy Trade off in Auditory Detection1 , 2022 .

[22]  J T Todd,et al.  Implications of a transient-sustained dichotomy for the measurement of human performance. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  R. Watt,et al.  A theory of the primitive spatial code in human vision , 1985, Vision Research.

[24]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—II. Single target displacements , 1979, Vision Research.

[25]  Marilyn L Shaw,et al.  Attending to multiple sources of information: I. The integration of information in decision making , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  J E Hoffman,et al.  Search through a sequentially presented visual display , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  B G Breitmeyer,et al.  Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing. , 1976, Psychological review.

[28]  Charles W. Eriksen,et al.  Selective attention and serial processing in briefly presented visual displays , 1971 .

[29]  R. J. Watt,et al.  An outline of the primal sketch in human vision , 1987, Pattern Recognit. Lett..

[30]  W K Estes,et al.  A DETECTION METHOD AND PROBABILISTIC MODELS FOR ASSESSING INFORMATION PROCESSING FROM BRIEF VISUAL DISPLAYS , 1964 .

[31]  J. Stone,et al.  Properties of cat retinal ganglion cells: a comparison of W-cells with X- and Y-cells. , 1974, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Organization of monkey superior colliculus: enhanced visual response of superficial layer cells. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  William K. Estes,et al.  Reaction time in relation to display size and correctness of response in forced-choice visual signal detection , 1966 .

[35]  Howard S. Bashinski,et al.  Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[36]  P Bertelson,et al.  The Time Course of Preparation* , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[39]  John R. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and visual attention , 1985 .

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  Sustained Concentration: Passive Filtering or Active Orienting? , 2019, Preparatory States & Processes.

[41]  M. R. Houck,et al.  Conjunction of color and form without attention: evidence from an orientation-contingent color aftereffect. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  M. R. Houck,et al.  The role of attentional resources in automatic detection , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  George Sperling,et al.  The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .

[44]  M. Manosevitz,et al.  High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , 2022 .

[45]  E. Averbach,et al.  Short-term memory in vision , 1961 .