Judgments of relative time-to-contact of more than two approaching objects: Toward a method
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Regan. The divergence of velocity and visual processing. , 1993, Perception.
[2] N. Graham. Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .
[3] Julian Hochberg,et al. Representation of motion and space in video and cinematic displays , 1986 .
[4] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[5] ADAPTATION AND POP-OUT IN THE ANALYSIS OF DIVERGENCE-SENSITIVE MECHANISMS , 1993 .
[6] W H Teichner,et al. Visual search for simple targets. , 1974, Psychological bulletin.
[7] Eugene Galanter,et al. Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .
[8] W Schiff,et al. Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] D. Regan,et al. Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to contact and for rate of angular expansion , 1993, Vision Research.
[10] J. Tresilian. Perceptual Information for the Timing of Interceptive Action , 1990, Perception.
[11] F. Previc,et al. Visual search asymmetries in three-dimensional space , 1993, Vision Research.
[12] D. M. Green,et al. Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .
[13] J. Pellegrino,et al. Perceptual and cognitive factors governing performance in comparative arrival-time judgments. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .
[15] Robert G Pachella,et al. The Interpretation of Reaction Time in Information-Processing Research 1 , 1973, Human Information Processing.
[16] P R DeLucia,et al. Cognitive motion extrapolation and cognitive clocking in prediction motion task. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] B. Frost,et al. Time to collision is signalled by neurons in the nucleus rotundus of pigeons , 1992, Nature.
[19] R J Jagacinski,et al. Quantifying the cognitive trajectories of extrapolated movements. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[21] James T. Townsend,et al. The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .
[22] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Sensitivity To Three-Dimensional Orientation in Visual Search , 1990 .
[23] G J Andersen,et al. Active control in interrupted dynamic spatial orientation: The detection of orientation change , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] J. Tresilian,et al. Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation: Analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgment tasks , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[25] W Schiff,et al. Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.
[26] J T Todd,et al. Visual information about moving objects. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Readings in mathematical psychology , 1963 .
[28] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A revised table of d’ for M-alternative forced choice , 1979 .
[29] J. Palmer. Attentional limits on the perception and memory of visual information. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] Eileen Kowler,et al. Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception , 1986, Vision Research.
[31] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[32] J. Tresilian. Empirical and theoretical issues in the perception of time to contact. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] Don McNicol,et al. A Primer of Signal Detection Theory , 1976 .
[34] Jon Driver,et al. Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel , 1988, Nature.
[35] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[36] Patricia R. Delucia,et al. Effects of Pictorial Relative Size and Ground-Intercept Information on Judgments about Potential Collisions in Perspective Displays , 1995, Hum. Factors.
[37] Irwin Pollack. Message Probability and Message Reception , 1963 .
[38] O. Braddick,et al. Serial Search for Targets Defined by Divergence or Deformation of Optic Flow , 1991, Perception.
[39] Norma Graham,et al. Detection and identification of spatial frequency: models and data , 1984, Vision Research.
[40] James W. Pellegrino,et al. The ability to reason about movement in the visual field , 1988 .
[41] W. Simpson,et al. Depth Discrimination from Optic Flow , 1988, Perception.
[42] M. Kaiser,et al. Optical specification of time-to-passage: Observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993 .
[43] M. Cheal,et al. Attention in visual search: Multiple search classes , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[44] N. Cowan. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.
[45] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control of self-motion: size-arrival effects on collision avoidance. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] A Treisman,et al. Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.
[47] T. Marill. Detection theory and psychophysics , 1956 .
[48] R J Bootsma,et al. Visual information about time-to-collision between two objects. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] I. Pollack. Message Uncertainty and Message Reception , 1959 .
[50] H. Barlow. The efficiency of detecting changes of density in random dot patterns , 1978, Vision Research.