Bayesian foraging with only two patch types

model the optimal Bayesian foraging strategy in environments with only two patch qualities. That is, all patches either belong to one rich type, or to one poor type. This has been a situation created in several foraging experiments. In contrast, previous theories of Bayesian foraging have dealt with prey distributions where patches may belong to one out of a large range of qualities (binomial, Poisson and negative binomial distributions). This study shows that two-patch systems have some unique properties. One qualitative difference is that in many cases it will be possible for a Bayesian forager to gain perfect information about patch quality. As soon as it has found more than the number of prey items that should be available in a poor patch, it "knows" that it is in a rich patch. The model generates at least three testable predictions. 1) The distribution of giving-up densities, GUDs, should be bimodal in rich patches, when rich patches are rare in the environment. This is because the optimal strategy is then devoted to using the poor patches correctly, at the expense of missing a large fraction of the few rich patches available. 2) There should be a negative relation between GUD and search time in poor patches, when rich patches are much more valuable than poor. This is because the forager gets good news about potential patch quality from finding some food. It therefore accepts a lower instantaneous intake rate, making it more resistant against runs of bad luck, decreasing the risk of discarding rich patches. 3) When the energy gains required to remain in the patch are high (such as under high predation risk), the overuse of poor patches and the underuse of rich increases. This is because less information about patch quality is gained if leaving at high intake rates (after short times). The predictions given by this model may provide a much needed and effective conceptual framework for testing (both in the lab and the field) whether animals are using Bayesian assessment. (Less)

[1]  O. Olsson,et al.  Optimal Bayesian foraging policies and prey population dynamics-some comments on Rodriguez-Girones and Vasquez. , 2000, Theoretical population biology.

[2]  Thomas J. Valone,et al.  Bayesian and prescient assessment: foraging with pre-harvest information , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  B. Grossi,et al.  On the value of information: studying changes in patch assessment abilities through learning , 2006 .

[4]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[5]  Richard F. Green A simpler, more general method of finding the optimal foraging strategy for Bayesian birds , 2006 .

[6]  Ola Olsson,et al.  The foraging benefits of information and the penalty of ignorance , 2006 .

[7]  J. McNamara Optimal patch use in a stochastic environment , 1982 .

[8]  Richard F. Green,et al.  Stochastic Models of Optimal Foraging , 1987 .

[9]  Thomas J. Valone,et al.  Are animals capable of Bayesian updating? An empirical review , 2006 .

[10]  Ola Olsson,et al.  The survival-rate-maximizing policy for Bayesian foragers: wait for good news , 1998 .

[11]  I. Kiss,et al.  Parasite strain coexistence in a heterogeneous host population , 2006 .

[12]  Richard F. Green,et al.  Bayesian birds: A simple example of Oaten's stochastic model of optimal foraging , 1980 .

[13]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  Patch Assessment in Fox Squirrels: The Role of Resource Density, Patch Size, and Patch Boundaries , 1996, The American Naturalist.

[14]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk, and competition , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[15]  A. Oaten,et al.  Optimal foraging in patches: a case for stochasticity. , 1977, Theoretical population biology.

[16]  Thomas J. Valone,et al.  Measuring Patch Assessment Abilities of Desert Granivores , 1989 .

[17]  Y. Iwasa,et al.  Prey Distribution as a Factor Determining the Choice of Optimal Foraging Strategy , 1981, The American Naturalist.

[18]  Richard F. Green,et al.  Stopping Rules for Optimal Foragers , 1984, The American Naturalist.

[19]  Theunis Piersma,et al.  Incompletely Informed Shorebirds That Face a Digestive Constraint Maximize Net Energy Gain When Exploiting Patches , 2003, The American Naturalist.

[20]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Downy Woodpecker Foraging Behavior: Efficient Sampling in Simple Stochastic Environments , 1984 .

[21]  Ola Olsson,et al.  Gain curves in depletable food patches: A test of five models with European starlings , 2001 .

[22]  Ola Olsson,et al.  Gaining ecological information about Bayesian foragers through their behaviour. II. A field test with woodpeckers , 1999 .

[23]  E. Charnov Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.

[24]  Thomas J. Valone,et al.  Information for patch assessment: a field investigation with black-chinned hummingbirds , 1992 .

[25]  Frederick R. Adler,et al.  Departure time versus departure rate: How to forage optimally when you are stupid , 1999 .

[26]  A. A. Milne,et al.  プーさんのおはなしえほん : Winnie-the-Pooh , 2006 .

[27]  Ola Olsson,et al.  Gaining ecological information about Bayesian foragers through their behaviour. I. Models with predictions , 1999 .

[28]  T. Valone,et al.  Foraging under Multiple Costs: The Importance of Predation, Energetic, and Assessment Error Costs to a Desert Forager , 1999 .

[29]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  Long- and short-term state-dependent foraging under predation risk: an indication of habitat quality , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  O. Olsson,et al.  Daily foraging routines and feeding effort of a small bird feeding on a predictable resource , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  Joel s. Brown,et al.  Testing values of crested porcupine habitats by experimental food patches , 1990, Oecologia.