Performance of Music Elevates Pain Threshold and Positive Affect: Implications for the Evolutionary Function of Music

It is well known that music arouses emotional responses. In addition, it has long been thought to play an important role in creating a sense of community, especially in small scale societies. One mechanism by which it might do this is through the endorphin system, and there is evidence to support this claim. Using pain threshold as an assay for CNS endorphin release, we ask whether it is the auditory perception of music that triggers this effect or the active performance of music. We show that singing, dancing and drumming all trigger endorphin release (indexed by an increase in post-activity pain tolerance) in contexts where merely listening to music and low energy musical activities do not. We also confirm that music performance results in elevated positive (but not negative) affect. We conclude that it is the active performance of music that generates the endorphin high, not the music itself. We discuss the implications of this in the context of community bonding mechanisms that commonly involve dance and music-making.

[1]  Michael C. Corballis Evolution of Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach , 2006 .

[2]  Cathy H. McKinney,et al.  The Effect of Selected Classical Music and Spontaneous Imagery on Plasma β-Endorphin , 1997, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[3]  C. Hatton,et al.  Music interventions for people with dementia: a review of the literature , 2004, Aging & mental health.

[4]  F. Bloom The endorphins: a growing family of pharmacologically pertinent peptides. , 1983, Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology.

[5]  R. Hilliard,et al.  The effects of music therapy on the quality and length of life of people diagnosed with terminal cancer. , 2003, Journal of music therapy.

[6]  G. Eifert,et al.  The effects of running and meditation on beta-endorphin, corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol in plasma, and on mood , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[7]  R. Depue,et al.  A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. , 2005, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[8]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[9]  Scott S. Wiltermuth,et al.  Synchrony and Cooperation , 2009, Psychological science.

[10]  Patrick Chiu,et al.  Music Therapy: Loud Noise or Soothing Notes? , 2003 .

[11]  L. Stein,et al.  Enkephalin may mediate euphoria and drive-reduction reward , 1977, Nature.

[12]  E. Keverne,et al.  Genes, brains and mammalian social bonds. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[13]  J. Roederer The Search for a Survival Value of Music , 1984 .

[14]  Steven C. Rockwell,et al.  Does humor facilitate coping with physical discomfort? , 1993 .

[15]  Till Sprenger,et al.  The runner's high: opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  A. Frantz,et al.  The effect of running on plasma beta-endorphin. , 1981, Life sciences.

[17]  R. Koeppe,et al.  Regulation of human affective responses by anterior cingulate and limbic mu-opioid neurotransmission. , 2003, Archives of general psychiatry.

[18]  David Watson,et al.  The PANAS-X manual for the positive and negative affect schedule , 1994 .

[19]  Dunbar Rim.,et al.  Mind the gap; or why humans are not just great apes , 2008 .

[20]  N. Wallin,et al.  Biomusicology: Neurophysiological, Neuropsychological and Evolutionary Perspectives on the Origins and Purposes of Music , 1992 .

[21]  R I M Dunbar,et al.  Bridging the bonding gap: the transition from primates to humans , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  Captured by motion: Dance, action understanding, and social cognition , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[23]  Eric B. Keverne,et al.  Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships , 1989, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[24]  Christian Dina,et al.  AVPR1a and SLC6A4 Gene Polymorphisms Are Associated with Creative Dance Performance , 2005, PLoS genetics.

[25]  Jaak Panksepp,et al.  Brain Substrates of Infant–Mother Attachment: Contributions of Opioids, Oxytocin, and Norepinephrine , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[26]  Rehydration and dementia: a review of the literature , 2006 .

[27]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  The brain opioid theory of social attachment: a review of the evidence , 2011 .

[29]  D. Franchini,et al.  Neuroendocrine responses of healthy volunteers to 'techno-music': relationships with personality traits and emotional state. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[30]  Gunter Hempelmann,et al.  β-endorphin (1-31) in the plasma of male volunteers undergoing physical exercise , 2000, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[31]  E. Durkheim FROM THE ELEMENTARY FORMS OF THE RELIGIOUS LIFE , 1996, The New Economic Sociology.

[32]  V. Turner,et al.  The Ritual Process. Structure and Anti-Structure , 1978 .

[33]  L. Jamner,et al.  Repressive/defensive coping, endogenous opioids and health: how a life so perfect can make you sick , 1999, Psychiatry Research.

[34]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Pattern and Process in Cultural Evolution , 2009 .

[35]  Hans-Georg Buchholz,et al.  Basal opioid receptor binding is associated with differences in sensory perception in healthy human subjects: A [18F]diprenorphine PET study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  G. Skrinar,et al.  Release of beta endorphin and met-enkephalin during exercise in normal women: response to training. , 1984, British medical journal.

[37]  Daniel J. Levitin,et al.  The rewards of music listening: Response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system , 2005, NeuroImage.

[38]  S. Hosztafi,et al.  Endogenous morphine. , 1995, Pharmacological research.

[39]  S. Vanderark,et al.  Cortisol, Biochemical, and Galvanic Skin Responses to Music Stimuli of Different Preference Values by College Students in Biology and Music , 1993, Perceptual and motor skills.

[40]  M. Birmingham,et al.  The human story , 2005, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[41]  K. T. Francis,et al.  Plasma levels of catecholamines, cortisol, and beta-endorphins in male athletes after running 26.2, 6, and 2 miles. , 1983, The Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness.

[42]  S. Vanderark,et al.  Biochemical and Galvanic Skin Responses to Music Stimuli by College Students in Biology and Music , 1992, Perceptual and motor skills.

[43]  Patrick J. O’Connor,et al.  LESSONS IN EXERCISE NEUROBIOLOGY: THE CASE OF ENDORPHINS , 2009 .

[44]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  G. Skrinar,et al.  Exercise and the endogenous opioids. , 1981, The New England journal of medicine.

[46]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Modularity of music processing , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[48]  A. Goldstein Thrills in response to music and other stimuli , 1980 .

[49]  N. Kalin,et al.  The central and peripheral opioid peptides. Their relationships and functions. , 1983, The Psychiatric clinics of North America.

[50]  Stefan Agamanolis,et al.  Exertion interfaces: sports over a distance for social bonding and fun , 2003, CHI '03.

[51]  W. Freeman,et al.  A neurobiological role of music in social bonding , 2000 .

[52]  M. Schilthuizen The darting game in snails and slugs. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[53]  R I M Dunbar,et al.  Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[54]  D. Huron,et al.  Is Music an Evolutionary Adaptation? , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[55]  L. Chlan,et al.  Effectiveness of a music therapy intervention on relaxation and anxiety for patients receiving ventilatory assistance. , 1998, Heart & lung : the journal of critical care.

[56]  C. Krumhansl An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. , 1997, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[57]  A Steptoe,et al.  Acute effects of aerobic exercise on mood. , 1988, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[58]  Joshua A. Bueller,et al.  Regional Mu Opioid Receptor Regulation of Sensory and Affective Dimensions of Pain , 2001, Science.

[59]  C. Macrae,et al.  The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception , 2009 .

[60]  B. Ehrenreich,et al.  Dancing in the Streets , 2006 .

[61]  P. Adler,et al.  Relaxation and music to reduce postsurgical pain. , 2008, Journal of advanced nursing.

[62]  Nicola Knight,et al.  Rowers' high: behavioural synchrony is correlated with elevated pain thresholds , 2010, Biology Letters.