Relational frame theory : finding its historical and philosophical roots and reflecting upon its future development : an introduction to part II

[1]  S. Hayes,et al.  Mastering the Clinical Conversation: Language as Intervention , 2015 .

[2]  Ian Stewart,et al.  Antecedent and consequential control of derived instruction-following. , 2014, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Extinction of aversive eliciting functions as an analog of exposure to conditioned fear: Does it alter avoidance responding? , 2013 .

[4]  S. Hayes,et al.  Contextual Behavioral Science: Creating a science more adequate to the challenge of the human condition , 2012 .

[5]  D. Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Holding on to our functional roots when exploring new intellectual islands: A voyage through implicit cognition research , 2012 .

[6]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  A Sketch of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Relational Elaboration and Coherence (REC) Model , 2010 .

[7]  I. Stewart,et al.  The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP ) and the Malleability of Ageist Attitudes , 2009 .

[8]  Carmen Luciano,et al.  The role of multiple-exemplar training and naming in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. , 2007, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[9]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Establishing Relational Responding in Accordance with More-than and Less-than as Generalized Operant Behavior in Young Children , 2004 .

[10]  Ian Stewart,et al.  Relational Frame Theory and Analogical Reasoning: Empirical Investigations , 2004 .

[11]  O Healy,et al.  Derived relational responding as generalized operant behavior. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[12]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Explaining Complex Behavior: Two Perspectives on the Concept Of Generalized Operant Classes , 2000 .

[13]  D. Barnes,et al.  Analyzing derived stimulus relations requires more than the concept of stimulus class. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[14]  B. Roche,et al.  A transformation of respondently conditioned stimulus function in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[15]  D. Barnes,et al.  A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more than, and less than. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[16]  S. Hayes,et al.  Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[17]  M Sidman,et al.  Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. , 1971, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[18]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Effects of an acceptance/defusion intervention on experimentally induced generalized avoidance: a laboratory demonstration. , 2014, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[19]  Ian Stewart,et al.  The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: Exploring the Impact of Private Versus Public Contexts and the Response Latency Criterion on Pro-White and Anti-Black Stereotyping Among White Irish Individuals , 2010 .

[20]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Three Chronometric Indices of Relational Responding as Predictors of Performance on a Brief Intelligence Test: The Importance of Relational Flexibility , 2009 .

[21]  R. Whelan,et al.  Citation Analysis of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior: 1984–2004 , 2006, The Behavior analyst.

[22]  Dermot Barnes-Holmes,et al.  Relational frame theory and Skinner’s Verbal Behavior: A possible synthesis , 2000, The Behavior analyst.