How the lack of visuomotor feedback affects even the early stages of goal-directed pointing movements

Pointing movements made with a hidden cursor from the center of gaze to a stationary, visible target overshot the actual target location. The systematic error decreased when the final cursor location from the previous trial was shown, which likely led to the creation of an internal sensorimotor model of movement. However, the putative model had a short memory, and could not substitute for on-line visuomotor feedback on subsequent trials. Contrary to common belief, the effect of a lack of visuomotor feedback was seen even in the early acceleration stage of the movement trajectory. Unchecked in the absence of visual monitoring, the acceleration stage of the movement lasted longer, as was evidenced by the significantly larger value of the peak cursor speed. Moreover, the speed peaked much later in the course of the movement. Speed declined more rapidly thereafter. Consequently, the delayed deceleration stage lasted far less than the acceleration stage. In the absence of visual feedback, the shift rightward in time of the peak speed position (PSP) in relation to total movement duration and other changes in the trajectory imply that visual feedback must play a significant role in determining when acceleration ceases (dV/dt=0), and argue against the traditional notion that visuomotor feedback is unavailable until the later stages of movement. Moreover, our data suggest that non-visual modalities, e.g., proprioception, may be too slow to make up for the absence of vision.

[1]  H. P. Birmingham,et al.  Studies of tracking behavior; the acceleration pattern of quick manual corrective responses. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  R. G. Lee,et al.  Motor Responses to Sudden Limb Displacements in Primates with Specific CNS Lesions and in Human Patients with Motor System Disorders , 1975, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[3]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  J F Soechting,et al.  A coordinate system for the synthesis of visual and kinesthetic information , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[6]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Localization of objects in the peripheral visual field , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[9]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: role of velocity and position information. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[12]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  D. Wolpert Computational approaches to motor control , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[15]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[17]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Predicting the Consequences of Our Own Actions: The Role of Sensorimotor Context Estimation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[19]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The effect of visuomotor displacements on arm movement paths , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Relative mislocalization of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina , 1999, Nature.

[22]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Changing objects lead briefly flashed ones , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.