Walking through an aperture with visual information obtained at a distance

The present study addressed whether visual information about the width of an aperture, obtained at a distance, would be sufficient to guide walking through the aperture without collision. For this purpose, we asked twelve young participants to walk while holding a 66-cm horizontal bar (bar length needs to be considered in order to perceive space necessary for crossing) and pass through an aperture without vision from 3 m in front of the aperture. Participants performed the tasks under each of four visual conditions, which differed in how vision was available: observation for 1.5 s while standing (static vision), observation during two forward steps and stopping (dynamic vision), observation during two forward steps and not stopping (dynamic vision with nonstop walking), and full vision. The results showed that, for narrow apertures (the widths were 0.8 and 1.0 times the bar length), the rate of collision without vision was about 40–50 %. This was mainly due to the maladaptive planning of body rotation. For the aperture 1.0 times the bar length, the percentage of trials with no body rotation was high, suggesting that at least some participants underestimated the space necessary for crossing. The location at which maximum body rotation occurred became farther from the obstacle, which may have been related to decreased movement speed. The availability of dynamic visual sampling during two forward steps did not contribute to improving collision avoidance. These results suggest that, while fundamental locomotor patterns are maintained even without online vision, both the underestimation of space required for crossing and the lack of fine-tuning of behavior prior to crossing increased collision rates.

[1]  F Danion,et al.  Veering in human locomotion: the role of the effectors , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[2]  J. B. J. Smeets,et al.  Grasping an object comfortably: orientation information is held in memory , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Can perception of aperture passability be improved immediately after practice in actual passage? Dissociation between walking and wheelchair use , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Perception of spatial requirements for wheelchair locomotion in experienced users with tetraplegia. , 2009, Journal of physiological anthropology.

[5]  Aftab E Patla,et al.  The influence of multiple obstacles in the travel path on avoidance strategy. , 2002, Gait & posture.

[6]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension , 1994, Current Biology.

[7]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Understanding the contribution of binocular vision to the control of adaptive locomotion , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Michael E. Cinelli,et al.  Young and older adults use body-scaled information during a non-confined aperture crossing task , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Renato Moraes,et al.  The effects of distant and on-line visual information on the control of approach phase and step over an obstacle during locomotion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Michael Greig,et al.  Any way you look at it, successful obstacle negotiation needs visually guided on-line foot placement regulation during the approach phase , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[11]  Michael E Cinelli,et al.  Is the Critical Point for Aperture Crossing Adapted to the Person-Plus-Object System? , 2014, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Directional bias in the body while walking through a doorway: its association with attentional and motor factors , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  M. Goodale,et al.  Obstacle avoidance during locomotion is unaffected in a patient with visual form agnosia , 1996, Neuroreport.

[15]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: Sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  John M. Franchak,et al.  Gut estimates: Pregnant women adapt to changing possibilities for squeezing through doorways , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[17]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  Michael E. R. Nicholls,et al.  Rightward collisions and their association with pseudoneglect , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  M. Beudel,et al.  Interruption of visually perceived forward motion in depth evokes a cortical activation shift from spatial to intentional motor regions , 2010, Brain Research.

[20]  M. Anthony Lewis,et al.  Strategies and determinants for selection of alternate foot placement during human locomotion: influence of spatial and temporal constraints , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Johannes Rennig,et al.  Memory-guided reaching in a patient with visual hemiagnosia , 2016, Cortex.

[22]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Perceiving Affordances for Aperture Crossing for the Person-Plus-Object System , 2005 .

[23]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Big People, Little World: The Body Influences Size Perception , 2009, Perception.

[24]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Rule for Scaling Shoulder Rotation Angles while Walking through Apertures , 2012, PloS one.

[25]  Michael E. Cinelli,et al.  Locomotion through apertures when wider space for locomotion is necessary: adaptation to artificially altered bodily states , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[27]  Aftab E Patla,et al.  Strategies used to walk through a moving aperture. , 2008, Gait & posture.

[28]  C. Richards,et al.  The negotiation of stationary and moving obstructions during walking: anticipatory locomotor adaptations and preservation of personal space. , 2005, Motor control.

[29]  John M. Franchak,et al.  What infants know and what they do: perceiving possibilities for walking through openings. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[30]  David B. Elliott,et al.  When Is Visual Information Used to Control Locomotion When Descending a Kerb? , 2011, PloS one.

[31]  M. Nicholls,et al.  Things that go bump in the right: The effect of unimanual activity on rightward collisions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  The relationship between spatial cognition and walking trajectory for passing through a doorway: Evident in individuals with dominant right eye? , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Lori Ann Vallis,et al.  Action strategies of individuals during aperture crossing in nonconfined space , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Athletic experience influences shoulder rotations when running through apertures. , 2011, Human movement science.

[36]  Michael E Cinelli,et al.  The effects of narrow and elevated path walking on aperture crossing. , 2015, Human movement science.

[37]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Visual estimation of spatial requirements for locomotion in novice wheelchair users. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[38]  Michael E Cinelli,et al.  Action strategies of older adults walking through apertures. , 2011, Gait & posture.

[39]  A. Milner,et al.  A paradoxical improvement of misreaching in optic ataxia: new evidence for two separate neural systems for visual localization , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  B. Day,et al.  Insights into the neural control of locomotion from walking through doorways in Parkinson's disease , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  M. Goodale Transforming vision into action , 2011, Vision Research.

[42]  Michael E Cinelli,et al.  Does the passability of apertures change when walking through human versus pole obstacles? , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[43]  Aftab E Patla,et al.  Task-specific modulations of locomotor action parameters based on on-line visual information during collision avoidance with moving objects. , 2008, Human movement science.

[44]  Michael E. Cinelli,et al.  Action strategies used by children to avoid two vertical obstacles in non-confined space , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.