A Biologically Based Computational Model of Working Memory

FIVE CENTRAL FEATURES OF THE MODEL We define working memory as controlled processing involving active maintenance and/or rapid learning, where controlled processing is an emergent property of the dynamic interactions of multiple brain systems, but the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus (HCMP) are especially influential owing to their specialized processing abilities and their privileged locations within the processing hierarchy (both the PFC and HCMP are well connected with a wide range of brain areas, allowing them to influence behavior at a global level). The specific features of our model include: (1) A PFC specialized for active maintenance of internal contextual information that is dynamically updated and self-regulated, allowing it to bias (control) ongoing processing according to maintained information (e.g., goals, instructions, partial products). (2) An HCMP specialized for rapid learning of arbitrary information, which can be recalled in the service of controlled processing, whereas the posterior perceptual and motor cortex (PMC) exhibits slow, long-term learning that can efficiently represent accumulated knowledge and skills. (3) Control that emerges from interacting systems (PFC, HCMP, and PMC). (4) Dimensions that define continua of specialization in different brain systems: for example, robust active maintenance, fast versus slow learning. (5) Integration of biological and computational principles. Working memory is an intuitively appealing theoretical construct – perhaps deceptively so.

[1]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[2]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Neuron Activity Related to Short-Term Memory , 1971, Science.

[3]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[4]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[5]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .

[6]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[7]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[8]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[10]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[11]  D. Pandya,et al.  Architecture and frontal cortical connections of the premotor cortex (area 6) in the rhesus monkey , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[12]  D. Amaral,et al.  Memory and the Hippocampus , 1989 .

[13]  J. Changeux,et al.  A Simple Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function in Delayed-Response Tasks , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  D. Pandya,et al.  Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  John H. Byrne,et al.  Neural Models of Plasticity: Experimental and Theoretical Approaches , 1989 .

[16]  A. Newell Unified Theories of Cognition , 1990 .

[17]  Elso Arruda,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology and neuropsychology of the frontal lobo , 1990 .

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[19]  J. Cohen,et al.  Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. , 1992, Psychological review.

[20]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  The neuropsychology of memory and aging. , 1992 .

[21]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[22]  F. Craik,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 1992 .

[23]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[24]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic "scotomas" , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  Mark S. Seidenberg Connectionist Models and Cognitive Theory , 1993 .

[26]  W. Schultz,et al.  Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Damasio,et al.  The frontal lobes , 1993 .

[28]  D. Zipser,et al.  A spiking network model of short-term active memory , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Computational Model of Prefrontal Cortex Function , 1994, NIPS.

[30]  Susan E. Gathercole,et al.  Neuropsychology and working memory: A review. , 1994 .

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[32]  R. Desimone,et al.  Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.

[33]  F. Craik,et al.  Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: positron emission tomography findings. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[35]  R. Davidson,et al.  Affective neuroscience: the emergence of a discipline , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[36]  A. Shimamura,et al.  Impaired use of organizational strategies in free recall following frontal lobe damage , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  P. Dayan,et al.  A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Daneman,et al.  Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  Richard D. Hichwa,et al.  A neural basis for lexical retrieval , 1996, Nature.

[40]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  The classic supplementary motor area is formed by two independent areas. , 1996, Advances in neurology.

[43]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[44]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A preliminary theory of the interactions between prefrontal cortex and hippocampus that contribute to planning and prospective memory. , 1996 .

[45]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[47]  A. Miyake,et al.  The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: an individual differences approach. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[48]  J. Cohen,et al.  Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context. A test of a theoretical model. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[49]  M. Petrides,et al.  Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[50]  Randall C. O'Reilly,et al.  Biologically Plausible Error-Driven Learning Using Local Activation Differences: The Generalized Recirculation Algorithm , 1996, Neural Computation.

[51]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[53]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[54]  Jürgen Schmidhuber,et al.  Long Short-Term Memory , 1997, Neural Computation.

[55]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A Hippocampal Model of Recognition Memory , 1997, NIPS.

[56]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[57]  S C Rao,et al.  Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 1997, Science.

[58]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  D. B. Mitchell,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 2001 .