Should hand actions be observed when learning hand motor skills from instructional animations?

This study investigated whether the effectiveness of learning a hand-motor task through an instructional animation required observation of the hands or not. Cognitive load theory was used to predict that both animated conditions (with and without hands) would be equally effective, and that both animations would be superior to an equivalent static graphics presentation. 36 adults were randomly assigned to three groups (With-hands animation, No-hands animation, Statics graphics) and were required to learn how to tie two knots. Test results confirmed that both animations led to superior learning compared to the static presentation. However, the With-hands animation strategy had a further advantage in that it had higher instructional efficiency than the No-hands animation.

[1]  Nadine Marcus,et al.  The Transient Information Effect: Investigating the Impact of Segmentation on Spoken and Written text , 2012 .

[2]  Richard K. Lowe,et al.  Dynamic visualisations and learning , 2004 .

[3]  Fred Paas,et al.  Making instructional animations more effective: a cognitive load approach , 2007 .

[4]  C. Heyes,et al.  Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  Richard Mayer,et al.  Multimedia Learning , 2001, Visible Learning Guide to Student Achievement.

[6]  Dong-Hee Shin,et al.  An empirical evaluation of multi-media based learning of a procedural task , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  P. Chandler,et al.  Why Some Material Is Difficult to Learn , 1994 .

[9]  Ricky Curran,et al.  Do dynamic work instructions provide an advantage over static instructions in a small scale assembly task , 2010 .

[10]  L. Proteau,et al.  Cognitive Processes Underlying Observational Learning of Motor Skills , 1999 .

[11]  John Sweller,et al.  Instructional animations can be superior to statics when learning human motor skills , 2009, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[12]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[13]  Joel R. Levin,et al.  Educating the evolved mind : conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology , 2007 .

[14]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  When static media promote active learning: annotated illustrations versus narrated animations in multimedia instruction. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[15]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[16]  S. Schwan,et al.  The cognitive benefits of interactive videos: learning to tie nautical knots , 2004 .

[17]  Tamara van Gog,et al.  An expertise reversal effect of segmentation in learning from animated worked-out examples , 2011, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[18]  Paul Ayres,et al.  Learning hand manipulative tasks: When instructional animations are superior to equivalent static representations , 2009, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[19]  F. Paas,et al.  Instructional Efficiency: Revisiting the Original Construct in Educational Research , 2008 .

[20]  D. Leutner,et al.  Instructional animation versus static pictures: A meta-analysis , 2007 .

[21]  Fred G. W. C. Paas,et al.  The Efficiency of Instructional Conditions: An Approach to Combine Mental Effort and Performance Measures , 1992 .

[22]  U. Castiello,et al.  The Human Premotor Cortex Is 'Mirror' Only for Biological Actions , 2004, Current Biology.

[23]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  S. Ainsworth,et al.  Multiple Forms of Dynamic Representation. , 2004 .

[25]  Béatrice S. Hasler,et al.  Learner Control, Cognitive Load and Instructional Animation , 2007 .

[26]  Rolf Plötzner,et al.  A systematic characterisation of expository animations , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[27]  J. Sweller,et al.  Cognitive load theory, modality of presentation and the transient information effect. , 2011 .

[28]  R. Mayer,et al.  When learning is just a click away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages? , 2001 .

[29]  F. Paas,et al.  Can the cognitive load approach make instructional animations more effective , 2007 .

[30]  L. R. Peterson,et al.  Short-term retention of individual verbal items. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  Gregory Schraw,et al.  Conceptions of Efficiency: Applications in Learning and Problem Solving , 2010 .

[32]  Christian Keysers,et al.  The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[33]  John Sweller,et al.  The Mirror Neuron System and Observational Learning: Implications for the Effectiveness of Dynamic Visualizations , 2009 .

[34]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Animation: can it facilitate? , 2002, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[35]  Peter Gerjets,et al.  How temporal and spatial aspects of presenting visualizations affect learning about locomotion patterns , 2012 .

[36]  J. Sweller,et al.  Cognitive load theory, the transient information effect and e-learning , 2012 .

[37]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory , 2020, Encyclopedia of Education and Information Technologies.