Broad direction bandwidths for complex motion mechanisms
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M G Harris,et al. Independent Detectors for Expansion and Rotation, and for Orthogonal Components of Deformation , 2001, Perception.
[2] T. Meese,et al. Probability summation for multiple patches of luminance modulation , 2000, Vision Research.
[3] T.C.A. Freeman,et al. Unequal retinal and extra-retinal motion signals produce different perceived slants of moving surfaces , 2000, Vision Research.
[4] A. T. Smith,et al. Global motion adaptation , 2000, Vision Research.
[5] Cardinal Directions for Optic Flow , 2000 .
[6] D. Burr,et al. Cardinal directions for visual optic flow , 1999, Current Biology.
[7] A. Metha,et al. Enhanced motion aftereffect for complex motions , 1999, Vision Research.
[8] J. Frisby,et al. The ‘Ecological’ Probability Density Function for Linear Optic Flow: Implications for Neurophysiology , 1999, Perception.
[9] D. Burr,et al. Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.
[10] P J Bex,et al. Psychophysical evidence for a functional hierarchy of motion processing mechanisms. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[11] John M. Foley,et al. Analysis of the effect of pattern adaptation on pattern pedestal effects: A two-process model , 1997, Vision Research.
[12] T. Meese,et al. Computation of surface slant from optic flow: Orthogonal components of speed gradient can be combined , 1997, Vision Research.
[13] G. Orban,et al. Selectivity of Macaque MT/V5 Neurons for Surface Orientation in Depth Specified by Motion , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.
[14] Guy A. Orban,et al. Visual Processing in Macaque Area MT/V5 and Its Satellites (MSTd and MSTv) , 1997 .
[15] Robert J. Snowden,et al. The Effects of Adapting to Complex Motions: Position Invariance and Tuning to Spiral Motions , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[16] D Buckley,et al. Evidence for Good Recovery of Lengths of Real Objects Seen with Natural Stereo Viewing , 1996, Perception.
[17] Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel,et al. Sub-pixel accuracy: Psychophysical validation of an algorithm for fine positioning and movement of dots on visual displays , 1996, Vision Research.
[18] Kevin Gurney,et al. Rotation and Radial Motion Thresholds Support a Two-Stage Model of Differential-Motion Analysis , 1996, Perception.
[19] T. Meese,et al. Deformation extractors in human vision: Evidence from subthreshold summation experiments , 1996 .
[20] Tim S. Meese,et al. Speed gradients and the perception of surface slant: Analysis is two-dimensional not one-dimensional , 1995, Vision Research.
[21] D. Burr,et al. Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.
[22] R. Wurtz,et al. Response of monkey MST neurons to optic flow stimuli with shifted centers of motion , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[23] T S Meese,et al. Using the standard staircase to measure the point of subjective equality: A guide based on computer simulations , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[25] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. III. Modeling , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[26] J. M. Foley,et al. Human luminance pattern-vision mechanisms: masking experiments require a new model. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[27] M. Graziano,et al. Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[28] Kechen Zhang,et al. Emergence of Position-Independent Detectors of Sense of Rotation and Dilation with Hebbian Learning: An Analysis , 1999, Neural Computation.
[29] David R Badcock,et al. Asymmetries in the Sensitivity to Motion in Depth: A Centripetal Bias , 1993, Perception.
[30] H. Wilson,et al. Spatial frequency adaptation and contrast gain control , 1993, Vision Research.
[31] J. Raymond. Movement direction analysers: Independence and bandwidth , 1993, Vision Research.
[32] A. Sekuler. Simple-pooling of unidirectional motion predicts speed discrimination for looming stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.
[33] T. Freeman,et al. Human sensitivity to expanding and rotating motion: effects of complementary masking and directional structure , 1992, Vision Research.
[34] D. Burr,et al. Two-dimensional spatial and spatial-frequency selectivity of motion-sensitive mechanisms in human vision. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[35] Guy A. Orban,et al. The role of direction information in the perception of geometric optic flow components , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] N. Graham. Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .
[37] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[38] H. Rodman,et al. Coding of visual stimulus velocity in area MT of the macaque , 1987, Vision Research.
[39] J. Koenderink. Optic flow , 1986, Vision Research.
[40] S. McKee,et al. Statistical properties of forced-choice psychometric functions: Implications of probit analysis , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.
[41] J Allman,et al. Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.
[42] M J Morgan,et al. Vernier acuity predicted from changes in the light distribution of the retinal image. , 1985, Spatial vision.
[43] T. Albright. Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.
[44] R. Sekuler,et al. Detection and identification of moving targets , 1983, Vision Research.
[45] A. Watson. Summation of grating patches indicates many types of detector at one retinal location , 1982, Vision Research.
[46] J. Robson,et al. Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field , 1981, Vision Research.
[47] Robert Sekuler,et al. A two-dimensional analysis of direction-specific adaptation , 1980, Vision Research.
[48] R Sekuler,et al. Models of stimulus uncertainty in motion perception. , 1980, Psychological review.
[49] H R Wilson,et al. Further evidence for four mechanisms mediating vision at threshold: sensitivities to complex gratings and aperiodic stimuli. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[50] R. Sekuler,et al. Masking of motion by broadband and filtered directional noise , 1979 .
[51] D. Regan,et al. Looming detectors in the human visual pathway , 1978, Vision Research.
[52] Quick Rf. A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. , 1974 .
[53] G. B. Wetherill,et al. SEQUENTIAL ESTIMATION OF POINTS ON A PSYCHOMETRIC FUNCTION. , 1965, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.
[54] T. Cornsweet,et al. The staircrase-method in psychophysics. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.
[55] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.