Harsh childhood environmental characteristics predict exploitation and retaliation in humans

Across and within societies, people vary in their propensities towards exploitative and retaliatory defection in potentially cooperative interaction. We hypothesized that this variation reflects adaptive responses to variation in cues during childhood that life will be harsh, unstable and short—cues that probabilistically indicate that it is in one's fitness interests to exploit co-operators and to retaliate quickly against defectors. Here, we show that childhood exposure to family neglect, conflict and violence, and to neighbourhood crime, were positively associated for men (but not women) with exploitation of an interaction partner and retaliatory defection after that partner began to defect. The associations between childhood environment and both forms of defection for men appeared to be mediated by participants' endorsement of a ‘code of honour’. These results suggest that individual differences in mutual benefit cooperation are not merely due to genetic noise, random developmental variation or the operation of domain-general cultural learning mechanisms, but rather, might reflect the adaptive calibration of social strategies to local social–ecological conditions.

[1]  D. Nettle,et al.  Variation in Cooperative Behaviour within a Single City , 2011, PloS one.

[2]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Impulsive Choice and Altruistic Punishment Are Correlated and Increase in Tandem With Serotonin Depletion , 2010, Emotion.

[3]  David C. Atkins,et al.  Rethinking how family researchers model infrequent outcomes: a tutorial on count regression and zero-inflated models. , 2007, Journal of family psychology : JFP : journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association.

[4]  C. Parks,et al.  Social Value Orientation and Cooperation in Social Dilemmas: A Meta-Analysis , 2009 .

[5]  T. Tyler Policing in Black and White: Ethnic Group Differences in Trust and Confidence in the Police , 2005 .

[6]  Tim Clutton-Brock,et al.  Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies , 2009, Nature.

[7]  M Daly,et al.  What happens when the private sector plans hospital services for the NHS: three case studies under the private finance initiative , 1997, BMJ.

[8]  David B. Fogel,et al.  The Iterated Prisoner's Dilemma , 1998 .

[9]  Christopher J. Schreck,et al.  “I Ain't Gonna Let No One Disrespect Me” , 2006 .

[10]  Beth E. Molnar,et al.  REFINING THE MEASUREMENT OF EXPOSURE TO VIOLENCE (ETV) IN URBAN YOUTH , 2007 .

[11]  Fiery Cushman,et al.  Evolving the Psychological Mechanisms for Cooperation , 2005 .

[12]  L Cosmides,et al.  On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: the role of genetics and adaptation. , 1990, Journal of personality.

[13]  G. Oehlert A note on the delta method , 1992 .

[14]  R. Boyd,et al.  In Search of Homo Economicus: Behavioral Experiments in 15 Small- Scale Societies , 2001 .

[15]  A. B. Hollingshead,et al.  Four factor index of social status , 1975 .

[16]  J. Garber,et al.  Sex-specific pathways to early puberty, sexual debut, and sexual risk taking: tests of an integrated evolutionary-developmental model. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[17]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation , 2002, Neuron.

[18]  Michael E. Price,et al.  Patience is a virtue: cooperative people have lower discount rates , 2008 .

[19]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES The Influence of Mortality and Socioeconomic Status on Risk and Delayed Rewards: A Life History Theory Approach , 2022 .

[20]  Lisa Farmer,et al.  Family unpredictability, future discounting, and risk taking , 2008 .

[21]  R. Trivers The Evolution of Reciprocal Altruism , 1971, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[22]  J. Berry,et al.  Forgivingness, vengeful rumination, and affective traits. , 2005, Journal of personality.

[23]  J. Black-Michaud Cohesive Force : Feud in the Mediterranean and the Middle East , 1975 .

[24]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Culture does account for variation in game behavior , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  M. Daly,et al.  Carpe Diem: Adaptation and Devaluing the Future , 2005, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[26]  J. Henrich,et al.  Costly Punishment Across Human Societies , 2006, Science.

[27]  A. Raftery Bayesian Model Selection in Social Research , 1995 .

[28]  Paul E Mullen,et al.  Homicide , 2000, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[29]  Paul H. Harvey,et al.  Living fast and dying young: A comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals , 1990 .

[30]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The evolution of cooperation. , 1984, Science.

[31]  J. Henrich,et al.  Markets, Religion, Community Size, and the Evolution of Fairness and Punishment , 2010, Science.

[32]  Colin Camerer,et al.  “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[33]  M. Nowak,et al.  Tit for tat in heterogeneous populations , 1992, Nature.

[34]  B. Ellis Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: an integrated life history approach. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  C. Kubrin,et al.  Retaliatory Homicide: Concentrated Disadvantage and Neighborhood Culture , 2003 .

[36]  Fred E. Markowitz Socioeconomic disadvantage and violence: Recent research on culture and neighborhood control as explanatory mechanisms , 2003 .

[37]  Max M. Krasnow,et al.  Evolution of direct reciprocity under uncertainty can explain human generosity in one-shot encounters , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Matthew A. Wyczalkowski,et al.  Niche Construction through Cooperation: A Nonlinear Dynamics Contribution to Modeling Facets of the Evolutionary History in the Genus Homo , 2010, Current Anthropology.

[39]  Margo Wilson,et al.  Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: the young male syndrome , 1985, Ethology and Sociobiology.

[40]  F. Schmidt Meta-Analysis , 2008 .

[41]  Stephanie V. Rohdieck,et al.  Who Takes the most Revenge? Individual Differences in Negative Reciprocity Norm Endorsement , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[42]  C. Anderson,et al.  Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: a meta-analysis. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  J. Rodgers,et al.  Poverty and Behavior: Are Environmental Measures Nature and Nurture?☆☆☆★ , 1997 .

[44]  Ruth Mace,et al.  Demography and ecology drive variation in cooperation across human populations , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  J. Berry,et al.  Conciliatory gestures facilitate forgiveness and feelings of friendship by making transgressors appear more agreeable. , 2012, Journal of personality.