Are videogame training gains specific or general?

Many recent studies using healthy adults document enhancements in perception and cognition from playing commercial action videogames (AVGs). Playing action games (e.g., Call of Duty, Medal of Honor) is associated with improved bottom-up lower-level information processing skills like visual-perceptual and attentional processes. One proposal states a general improvement in the ability to interpret and gather statistical information to predict future actions which then leads to better performance across different perceptual/attentional tasks. Another proposal claims all the tasks are separately trained in the AVGs because the AVGs and laboratory tasks contain similar demands. We review studies of action and non-AVGs to show support for the latter proposal. To explain transfer in AVGs, we argue that the perceptual and attention tasks share common demands with the trained videogames (e.g., multiple object tracking (MOT), rapid attentional switches, and peripheral vision). In non-AVGs, several studies also demonstrate specific, limited transfer. One instance of specific transfer is the specific enhancement to mental rotation after training in games with a spatial emphasis (e.g., Tetris). In contrast, the evidence for transfer is equivocal where the game and task do not share common demands (e.g., executive functioning). Thus, the “common demands” hypothesis of transfer not only characterizes transfer effects in AVGs, but also non-action games. Furthermore, such a theory provides specific predictions, which can help in the selection of games to train human cognition as well as in the design of videogames purposed for human cognitive and perceptual enhancement. Finally this hypothesis is consistent with the cognitive training literature where most post-training gains are for tasks similar to the training rather than general, non-specific improvements.

[1]  H Yuji,et al.  Computer Games and Information-Processing Skills , 1996, Perceptual and motor skills.

[2]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[3]  Jutta Stahl,et al.  An ERP study of target competition: individual differences in functional impulsive behavior. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[4]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[5]  P. Jensen Computerized Training of Working Memory in Children With ADHD: A Randomized, Controlled TrialKlingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, et al (Karolinska Inst, Sweden; Göteborg Univ, Sweden; Linköping Univ, Sweden; et al) J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:177–186, 2005§ , 2006 .

[6]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  The Pervasive Problem With Placebos in Psychology , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[7]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[8]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  Working Memory Training May Increase Working Memory Capacity but Not Fluid Intelligence , 2013, Psychological science.

[9]  Lars Konzack Video Game Genres , 2015 .

[10]  N. Lavie,et al.  On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .

[11]  Salil H. Patel,et al.  Impact of instructed relevance on the visual ERP. , 2004, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[12]  Adam C. Oei,et al.  Enhancing Cognition with Video Games: A Multiple Game Training Study , 2013, PloS one.

[13]  Leonard A. Annetta,et al.  The gorilla in the room: The impacts of video-game play on visual attention , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[14]  M. Kossut,et al.  [Brain plasticity]. , 2000, Neurologia i neurochirurgia polska.

[15]  F. Gomez-Pinilla The influences of diet and exercise on mental health through hormesis , 2008, Ageing Research Reviews.

[16]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  A Baddeley,et al.  Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Improved Probabilistic Inference as a General Learning Mechanism with Action Video Games , 2010, Current Biology.

[19]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Attentional blink , 2009, Scholarpedia.

[20]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  DOOM'd to Switch: Superior Cognitive Flexibility in Players of First Person Shooter Games , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[21]  E. Thorndike,et al.  The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. (I). , 1901 .

[22]  D. Gopher,et al.  Transfer of Skill from a Computer Game Trainer to Flight , 1994 .

[23]  Karen M. Arnell,et al.  Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  I. Spence,et al.  Video Games and Spatial Cognition , 2010 .

[25]  Julie I. Cohen,et al.  Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Joseph D. Chisholm,et al.  Reduced attentional capture in action video game players , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[27]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[28]  A. Kramer,et al.  Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[29]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD--a randomized, controlled trial. , 2005, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[30]  Camarin E. Rolle,et al.  Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults , 2013, Nature.

[31]  D. Simons,et al.  The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[32]  Palvi Bhardwaj,et al.  Action video game players and deaf observers have larger Goldmann visual fields , 2010, Vision Research.

[33]  J. Pratt,et al.  Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition , 2007, Psychological science.

[34]  D. Gentile,et al.  The impact of video games on training surgeons in the 21st century. , 2007, Archives of surgery.

[35]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Improved top-down control reduces oculomotor capture: The case of action video game players , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[36]  L. Goossens,et al.  Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[37]  R. Mayer,et al.  Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. , 2002 .

[38]  R. Schmidt,et al.  New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .

[39]  U. Polat,et al.  Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Changes in cortical activity during mental rotation. A mapping study using functional MRI. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  M. D. Calero,et al.  Cognitive plasticity as a modulating variable on the effects of memory training in elderly persons. , 2007, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[42]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[43]  Lars Bäckman,et al.  Transfer of Learning After Updating Training Mediated by the Striatum , 2008, Science.

[44]  C. S. Green,et al.  Changes in search rate but not in the dynamics of exogenous attention in action videogame players , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[45]  C. Sanchez Enhancing visuospatial performance through video game training to increase learning in visuospatial science domains , 2011, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[46]  C. Lustig,et al.  Who Benefits From Memory Training? , 2007, Psychological science.

[47]  Ian Spence,et al.  Playing shooter and driving videogames improves top-down guidance in visual search , 2013, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[48]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Neural bases of selective attention in action video game players , 2012, Vision Research.

[49]  Daphne Bavelier,et al.  The effect of action video game experience on task-switching , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[50]  Monica Melby-Lervåg,et al.  Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[51]  P. Frensch,et al.  Effects of video game playing on measures of spatial performance: Gender effects in late adolescence. , 1994 .

[52]  Matthew S Cain,et al.  Action video game experience reduces the cost of switching tasks , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[53]  K L Shapiro,et al.  Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  Lynette J. Tippett,et al.  Just how expert are “expert” video-game players? Assessing the experience and expertise of video-game players across “action” video-game genres , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[55]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[56]  Jing Feng,et al.  Playing a First-person Shooter Video Game Induces Neuroplastic Change , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[58]  C. Chabris,et al.  Gorillas in Our Midst: Sustained Inattentional Blindness for Dynamic Events , 1999, Perception.

[59]  Roger W. Remington,et al.  Not so fast: Rethinking the effects of action video games on attentional capacity , 2011 .

[60]  D. Roenker,et al.  Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[61]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  Is working memory training effective? , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[62]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Action video games and informal education: Effects on strategies for dividing visual attention , 1994 .

[63]  M. Linn,et al.  Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.

[64]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Learning mnemonics: roles of aging and subtle cognitive impairment. , 1990, Psychology and aging.

[65]  A F Sanders,et al.  Some aspects of the selective process in the functional visual field. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[66]  Maayan Pereg,et al.  Task switching training effects are mediated by working-memory management , 2013 .

[67]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Neural Basis of Superior Performance of Action Videogame Players in an Attention-Demanding Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[69]  K. Murphy,et al.  Playing video games does not make for better visual attention skills , 2009 .

[70]  M. Buchsbaum,et al.  Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning , 1992 .

[71]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[72]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Response to memory training and individual differences in verbal intelligence and state anxiety. , 1988, The American journal of psychiatry.

[73]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  The Spatial Resolution of Visual Attention , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[74]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Working Memory Training Using Mental Calculation Impacts Regional Gray Matter of the Frontal and Parietal Regions , 2011, PloS one.

[75]  Sherif Karama,et al.  BMC Research Notes BioMed Central , 2008 .

[76]  C. S. Green,et al.  Brain plasticity through the life span: learning to learn and action video games. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[77]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Effect of video game practice on spatial skills in girls and boys , 1994 .

[78]  E. Mather Novelty, attention, and challenges for developmental psychology , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[79]  D. Levi,et al.  The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea , 1992, Vision Research.

[80]  Karen Murphy,et al.  Does Video Game Playing Improve Executive Functioning , 2006 .

[81]  Renjie Li,et al.  Perceptual Learning During Action Video Game Playing , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[82]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  On the role of verbalization during task set selection: Switching or serial order control? , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[83]  C S Green,et al.  Action-Video-Game Experience Alters the Spatial Resolution of Vision , 2007, Psychological science.

[84]  Á. Kristjánsson The case for causal influences of action videogame play upon vision and attention , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[85]  C. S. Green,et al.  Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players , 2006, Cognition.

[86]  J. Pratt,et al.  The effects of action video game experience on the time course of inhibition of return and the efficiency of visual search. , 2005, Acta psychologica.