Lexical selection in bilingual speech production does not involve language suppression.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Kroll,et al. Language Production in Bilinguals , 1997 .
[2] Albert Costa,et al. Lexical Selection in Bilinguals: Do Words in the Bilingual's Two Lexicons Compete for Selection? , 1999 .
[3] D. Green. Control, activation, and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals , 1986, Brain and Language.
[4] J. Kroll,et al. Tutorials in bilingualism : psycholinguistic perspectives , 1999 .
[5] N. Yeung,et al. Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] D. Green,et al. Lemma selection without inhibition of languages in bilingual speakers , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[7] J. Kroll,et al. Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations , 1994 .
[8] K. Forster,et al. The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming , 2004 .
[9] A. Caramazza,et al. Where Do Semantic Errors Come From? , 1990, Cortex.
[10] Matthew Finkbeiner. Bilingual lexical memory: Towards a psycholinguistic model of adult L2 lexical acquisition, representation, and processing , 2002 .
[11] G. D. Logan. Task Switching , 2022 .
[12] I. Biederman,et al. Mental set and mental shift revisited , 1976 .
[13] A. Treisman,et al. Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .
[14] A. D. Groot,et al. Language control in bilinguals: Monolingual tasks and simultaneous interpreting , 2006, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[15] D. Norman,et al. Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .
[16] David W. Green,et al. Towards a Model of L2 Comprehension and Production , 1993 .
[17] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[18] H A Whitaker,et al. The bilingual brain. , 1978, Archives of neurology.
[19] A. Allport,et al. Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection , 1999 .
[20] W. Heij. Selection Processes in Monolingual and Bilingual Lexical Access. , 2005 .
[21] D. Balota,et al. The attentional control of lexical processing pathways: Reversing the word frequency effect , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[22] S Monsell,et al. Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.
[23] M. Potter,et al. Lexical and conceptual representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals , 1984 .
[24] Kenneth I Forster,et al. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[25] Stephen Monsell,et al. The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] G S Dell,et al. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.
[27] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Now You See it, Now you Don't: On Turning Semantic Interference Into Facilitation in a Stroop-Like Task , 2006, Cortex.
[28] A. Caramazza,et al. Lexical access in bilingual speakers: What's the (hard) problem? , 2006, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[29] Ulrika Kahl,et al. [The bilingual brain]. , 2002, Lakartidningen.
[30] Albert Costa,et al. Lexical Access in Bilingual Speech Production: Evidence from Language Switching in Highly Proficient Bilinguals and L2 Learners. , 2004 .
[31] G. Schwartz,et al. Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .
[32] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[33] W. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Making Sense of Semantic Ambiguity: Semantic Competition in Lexical Access , 2002 .
[34] Stephen J. Lupker,et al. The semantic nature of response competition in the picture-word interference task , 1979 .
[35] A. Caramazza,et al. When more is less: a counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[36] G. Wylie,et al. Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. , 1999 .
[37] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[38] Donald A. Norman,et al. Attention to Action , 1986 .
[39] Christiane Fellbaum,et al. Book Reviews: WordNet: An Electronic Lexical Database , 1999, CL.
[40] R. R. Peterson,et al. Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .
[41] A. Caramazza. How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .
[42] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[43] John Macnamara,et al. The linguistic independence of bilinguals , 1967 .
[44] S. Cobb. Speech and Brain-Mechanisms. , 1960 .
[45] W. Poulisse,et al. First Language Use in Second Language Production , 1994 .
[46] Judith F. Kroll,et al. Number-of-translation norms for Dutch—English translation pairs: A new tool for examining language production , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[47] Michel Paradis,et al. Aspects of Bilingualism , 1978 .
[48] R. Schreuder,et al. The Bilingual lexicon , 1993 .
[49] A. Roelofs,et al. A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.
[50] B. Hommel,et al. Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.