Rhythm and Beat Perception in Motor Areas of the Brain

When we listen to rhythm, we often move spontaneously to the beat. This movement may result from processing of the beat by motor areas. Previous studies have shown that several motor areas respond when attending to rhythms. Here we investigate whether specific motor regions respond to beat in rhythm. We predicted that the basal ganglia and supplementary motor area (SMA) would respond in the presence of a regular beat. To establish what rhythm properties induce a beat, we asked subjects to reproduce different types of rhythmic sequences. Improved reproduction was observed for one rhythm type, which had integer ratio relationships between its intervals and regular perceptual accents. A subsequent functional magnetic resonance imaging study found that these rhythms also elicited higher activity in the basal ganglia and SMA. This finding was consistent across different levels of musical training, although musicians showed activation increases unrelated to rhythm type in the premotor cortex, cerebellum, and SMAs (pre-SMA and SMA). We conclude that, in addition to their role in movement production, the basal ganglia and SMAs may mediate beat perception.

[1]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[2]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. , 1995 .

[3]  C. Drake,et al.  The “Ticktock” of Our Internal Clock , 2003, Psychological science.

[4]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  Simon Grondin,et al.  Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  D J Povel,et al.  A theoretical framework for rhythm perception , 1984, Psychological research.

[7]  C. Drake Psychological Processes Involved in the Temporal Organization of Complex Auditory Sequences: Universal and Acquired Processes , 1998 .

[8]  S. Kotz,et al.  Non-motor basal ganglia functions: A review and proposal for a model of sensory predictability in auditory language perception , 2009, Cortex.

[9]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Do cortical and basal ganglionic motor areas use “motor programs” to control movement? , 1992 .

[10]  Peter Essens,et al.  Perception of Temporal Patterns , 1985 .

[11]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  Experimental Evidence for Synchronization to a Musical Beat in a Nonhuman Animal , 2009, Current Biology.

[12]  P. Brotchie,et al.  Motor function of the monkey globus pallidus. 2. Cognitive aspects of movement and phasic neuronal activity. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  H H Schulze,et al.  The detectability of local and global displacements in regular rhythmic patterns , 1978, Psychological research.

[14]  P J Essens,et al.  Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  P. Apicella,et al.  Influence of the predicted time of stimuli eliciting movements on responses of tonically active neurons in the monkey striatum , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  A. Daffertshofer,et al.  Tapping with intentional drift , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Mari Tervaniemi,et al.  :The Neurosciences and Music II: From Perception to Performance , 2007 .

[18]  D. Povel,et al.  Accents in equitone sequences , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Moving on Time: Brain Network for Auditory-Motor Synchronization is Modulated by Rhythm Complexity and Musical Training , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Miall,et al.  Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: evidence from neuroimaging , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[21]  P. Essens,et al.  Structuring temporal sequences: Comparison of models and factors of complexity , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  J. Tanji,et al.  The role of premotor cortex and the supplementary motor area in the temporal control of movement in man. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Peter Q. Pfordresher,et al.  Tracking Musical Patterns using Joint Accent Structure , 1997 .

[24]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Neural networks for the coordination of the hands in time. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  Multilevel coordination stability: integrated goal representations in simultaneous intra-personal and inter-agent coordination. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[26]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Timing, Storage, and Comparison of Stimulus Duration Engage Discrete Anatomical Components of a Perceptual Timing Network , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  H. Dinse,et al.  Assessment of sensorimotor cortical representation asymmetries and motor skills in violin players , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  W. Schady,et al.  The influence of external timing cues upon the rhythm of voluntary movements in Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[29]  Usha Goswami,et al.  Rhythmic motor entrainment in children with speech and language impairments: Tapping to the beat , 2009, Cortex.

[30]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  Spontaneous Motor Entrainment to Music in Multiple Vocal Mimicking Species , 2009, Current Biology.

[31]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Dynamic attending and responses to time. , 1989, Psychological review.

[32]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  R. Zatorre,et al.  When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Snyder,et al.  Pulse and Meter as Neural Resonance , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[35]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: a comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  Warren H Meck,et al.  Cortico-striatal Representation of Time in Animals and Humans This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Cognitive Neuroscience Edited Evidence from Patient Populations and Electrical Potentials Neuroimaging Evidence Using Fmri and Pet , 2022 .

[38]  Jun Tanji,et al.  Projections from the globus pallidus to the thalamic areas projecting to the dorsal area 6 of the macaque monkey: a multiple tracing study , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  Robert J Zatorre,et al.  The Role of Auditory and Premotor Cortex in Sensorimotor Transformations , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  J. Jonides,et al.  Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.

[41]  W. Meck,et al.  How music fills our emotions and helps us keep time , 2008 .

[42]  André Dufour,et al.  Evidence of beat perception via purely tactile stimulation , 2008, Brain Research.

[43]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[44]  J. Duncan,et al.  Encoding Strategies Dissociate Prefrontal Activity from Working Memory Demand , 2003, Neuron.

[45]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  Neuroscientific Investigations of Musical Rhythm: Recent Advances and Future Challenges , 2009 .

[46]  Richard M. Warren,et al.  Perception of acoustic sequences: Global integration versus temporal resolution. , 1993 .

[47]  P. Essens,et al.  Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  Jessica Phillips-Silver On the Meaning of Movement in Music, Development and the Brain , 2009 .

[49]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[50]  D. Levitin,et al.  Current Advances in the Cognitive Neuroscience of Music , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[51]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  The Role of the Basal Ganglia in Beat Perception , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[52]  Christian Gaser,et al.  Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: new evidence from fMRI , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  P. A. Lewis,et al.  Brain activity correlates differentially with increasing temporal complexity of rhythms during initialisation, synchronisation, and continuation phases of paced finger tapping , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Functional Neuroanatomy of Temporal Discrimination , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  C. Drake,et al.  The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending , 2000, Cognition.

[56]  Alan R. Palmer,et al.  A high-output, high-quality sound system for use in auditory fMRI , 1998, NeuroImage.

[57]  C. Drake,et al.  Tapping in Time with Mechanically and Expressively Performed Music , 2000 .

[58]  R. Ivry The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[59]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Interval and ordinal properties of sequences are associated with distinct premotor areas. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  J. Doyon,et al.  Dynamic Cortical and Subcortical Networks in Learning and Delayed Recall of Timed Motor Sequences , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  Studying Synchronization to a Musical Beat in Nonhuman Animals , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[62]  J A Obeso,et al.  Temporal discrimination is abnormal in Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  R B Ivry,et al.  Dissociable contributions of the prefrontal and neocerebellar cortex to time perception. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[64]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  The Neural Correlate of Speech Rhythm as Evidenced by Metrical Speech Processing , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Robust automated brain extraction , 2000, NeuroImage.

[66]  R. Parncutt A Perceptual Model of Pulse Salience and Metrical Accent in Musical Rhythms , 1994 .

[67]  H Pashler,et al.  Perception and production of brief durations: beat-based versus interval-based timing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  J. Tanji,et al.  Neuronal activity in the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas for temporal organization of multiple movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[70]  M. Molinari,et al.  Neurobiology of Rhythmic Motor Entrainment , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[71]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  Top‐Down Control of Rhythm Perception Modulates Early Auditory Responses , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[72]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  A. Nobre,et al.  Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[74]  R. Cusack,et al.  New Robust 3-D Phase Unwrapping Algorithms: Application to Magnetic Field Mapping and Undistorting Echoplanar Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[75]  P. Brotchie,et al.  Motor function of the monkey globus pallidus. 1. Neuronal discharge and parameters of movement. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[76]  L. Trainor,et al.  Feeling the Beat: Movement Influences Infant Rhythm Perception , 2005, Science.

[77]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[78]  Roberto Nicoletti,et al.  Mechanisms of perceptual timing: Beat-based or interval-based judgements? , 1989 .

[79]  A. Friederici,et al.  Time Perception and Motor Timing: A Common Cortical and Subcortical Basis Revealed by fMRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[80]  M. Grube,et al.  Metricality-enhanced temporal encoding and the subjective perception of rhythmic sequences , 2009, Cortex.

[81]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.

[82]  A. Owen,et al.  Prefrontal cortical involvement in verbal encoding strategies , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[83]  R. K. Simpson Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .

[84]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Effect of deviations from temporal expectations on tempo discrimination of isochronous tone sequences. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[85]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Motor Timing: A PET Study of Auditory and Visual Rhythm Reproduction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[86]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Changes in the Human Brain during Rhythm Learning , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  For effective sensorimotor processing must there be explicit representations and reconciliation of differing frames of reference? , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[88]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  The classic supplementary motor area is formed by two independent areas. , 1996, Advances in neurology.

[89]  J. Binder,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[90]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Timing functions of the supplementary motor area: an event-related fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[91]  I. Peretz,et al.  Contribution of different cortical areas in the temporal lobes to music processing. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[92]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Neural bases of individual differences in beat perception , 2009, NeuroImage.

[93]  S Lehéricy,et al.  Basal ganglia and supplementary motor area subtend duration perception: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[94]  Piet G. Vos,et al.  Perceived tempo change is dependent on base tempo and direction of change: Evidence for a generalized version of Schulze's (1978) internal beat model , 1997 .

[95]  O Hikosaka,et al.  Neural Representation of a Rhythm Depends on Its Interval Ratio , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[96]  W. D. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[97]  M. Brett,et al.  Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Cortex.

[98]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[99]  W. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[100]  Kurt Wiesenfeld,et al.  Neural correlates of the complexity of rhythmic finger tapping , 2003, NeuroImage.

[101]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Hearing in the Mind's Ear: A PET Investigation of Musical Imagery and Perception , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[102]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Moving on Time: Brain Network for Auditory-Motor Synchronization is Modulated by Rhythm Complexity and Musical Training , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[103]  F. Vidal,et al.  Functional Anatomy of the Attentional Modulation of Time Estimation , 2004, Science.

[104]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.