Cognitive conflict and inhibition in primed dichotic listening

In previous behavioral studies, a prime syllable was presented just prior to a dichotic syllable pair, with instructions to ignore the prime and report one syllable from the dichotic pair. When the prime matched one of the syllables in the dichotic pair, response selection was biased towards selecting the unprimed target. The suggested mechanism was that the prime was inhibited to reduce conflict between task-irrelevant prime processing and task-relevant dichotic target processing, and a residual effect of the prime inhibition biased the resolution of the conflict between the two targets. The current experiment repeated the primed dichotic listening task in an event-related fMRI setting. The fMRI data showed that when the task-irrelevant prime matched the task-relevant targets, activations in posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) and in right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) increased, which was considered to represent conflict and inhibition, respectively. Further, matching trials where the unprimed target was selected showed activation in right IFG, while matching trials where the primed target was selected showed activations in pMFC and left IFG, indicating the difference between inhibition-biased selection and unbiased selection.

[1]  S. Goldman,et al.  Attention-dependent changes of activation and connectivity in dichotic listening: Evidence from two studies , 2001, NeuroImage.

[2]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[4]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Dichotic listening reveals functional specificity in prefrontal cortex: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[5]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  R. Kahn,et al.  Language lateralization in schizophrenia, an fMRI study , 2001, Schizophrenia Research.

[7]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Determinants of negative priming. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Régine Kolinsky,et al.  Attention-Dependent Changes of Activation and Connectivity in Dichotic Listening , 2002, NeuroImage.

[10]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  Executive Brake Failure following Deactivation of Human Frontal Lobe , 2006 .

[11]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Non-invasive regime for language lateralization in right and left-handers by means of functional MRI and dichotic listening , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibitory Mechanisms of Neural and Cognitive Control: Applications to Selective Attention and Sequential Action , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[15]  Daniel S. O'Leary,et al.  A Positron Emission Tomography Study of Binaurally and Dichotically Presented Stimuli: Effects of Level of Language and Directed Attention , 1996, Brain and Language.

[16]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  The “Forced-Attention Paradigm” in Dichotic Listening to CV-Syllables: A Comparison Between Adults and Children , 1986, Cortex.

[17]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Effects of attention on dichotic listening: An 15O‐PET study , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[18]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Focused attention in a simple dichotic listening task: an fMRI experiment. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[19]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Activation of Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus during Response Inhibition across Response Modalities , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[21]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  A E Seiffert,et al.  Selective attention: a reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. , 1998, Psychological review.

[23]  K. Munhall,et al.  Attentional biases and the right-ear effect in dichotic listening , 1983, Brain and Language.

[24]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Lateralization of cognitive processes in the brain. , 2000, Acta psychologica.

[25]  D. Broadbent The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  A. Parkin,et al.  Reversed negative priming following frontal lobe lesions , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  D. Kimura Functional Asymmetry of the Brain in Dichotic Listening , 1967 .

[28]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  B. Pfleiderer,et al.  Visualization of Auditory Habituation by fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[30]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[31]  Jeffrey P. Toth,et al.  Dissociation of attentional processes in patients with focal frontal and posterior lesions , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Deanna M. Barch,et al.  Extracting core components of cognitive control , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Does dichotic listening probe temporal lobe functions? , 2002, Neurology.

[36]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Focused and Nonfocused Attention in Verbal and Emotional Dichotic Listening: An FMRI Study , 2001, Brain and Language.

[37]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[38]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Priming inhibits the right ear advantage in dichotic listening: Implications for auditory laterality , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Endogenous and exogenous control of attention in dichotic listening. , 2007, Neuropsychology.

[40]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Katya Rubia,et al.  Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection , 2003, NeuroImage.

[42]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[43]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.