Attention, Preference, and Identity in Music Listening by Middle School Students of Different Linguistic Backgrounds

Monolingual (English) and bilingual (English, Spanish) sixth-grade students (N = 60) from two urban school settings in the United States listened to three versions of the same song (English, Spanish, and instrumental). While listening to each example, students tapped a computer touch pad every time they became distracted from the music. After listening, students described the nature of their distractions, and rated each song version for preference and identity. Finally, students were asked to describe the reason for their preference decisions. Analysis revealed no significant difference between monolingual and bilingual students in the number of self-reported distractions. However, there were significant differences by song version, with fewer reported distractions for the instrumental version. Qualitative and quantitative differences in distractions were noted between monolingual and bilingual students. There was a significant positive correlation between identity and preference. Bilingual students preferred and identified most closely with the Spanish version; monolingual students preferred and identified most closely with the instrumental version.

[1]  Leif Finnas,et al.  How Can Musical Preferences Be Modified? A Research Review. , 1989 .

[2]  Carlos R. Abril No Hablo Inglés: Breaking the Language Barrier in Music Instruction , 2003 .

[3]  C. K. Madsen,et al.  The Effect of Manipulating a CRDI Dial on the Focus of Attention of Musicians/nonmusicians and Perceived Aesthetic Response , 2001 .

[4]  S. Morrison,et al.  A Comparison of Preference Responses of White and African-American Students to Musical versus Musical/Visual Stimuli , 1998 .

[5]  M. Slobin Subcultural Sounds: Micromusics of the West , 1993 .

[6]  Carlos R. Abril Functions of a National Anthem in Society and Education: A Sociocultural Perspective , 2007 .

[7]  P. Campbell Songs in Their Heads: Music and Its Meaning in Children's Lives , 2011 .

[8]  J. McCrary Ethnic Majority/Minority Status: Children's Interactions and Affective Responses to Music , 2000 .

[9]  John M. Geringer,et al.  A focus of attention model for meaningful listening , 2000 .

[10]  Ruth V. Brittin Children's Preference for Sequenced Accompaniments: The Influence of Style and Perceived Tempo , 2000 .

[11]  M. E. Stafford,et al.  Hispanic Children’S Recognition of Languages and Perceptions about Speakers of Spanish, English, and Chinese , 1997 .

[12]  Eric A. Fenkl Diversity and Society: Race, Ethnicity, and Gender , 2007 .

[13]  S. Schultz,et al.  MENC : The National Association for Music Education Children ' s Preference for Authentic versus Arranged Versions of World Music Recordings , 2007 .

[14]  B. Repp,et al.  Integration of melody and text in memory for songs , 1982, Cognition.

[15]  C. Victor Fung,et al.  Undergraduate Nonmusic Majors' World Music Preference and Multicultural Attitudes , 1994 .

[16]  Patricia K. Shehan Transfer of Preference from Taught to Untaught Pieces of Non-Western Music Genres , 1985 .

[17]  Paul J. Silvia,et al.  Can positive affect induce self-focused attention? Methodological and measurement issues , 2002 .

[18]  P. Greenfield,et al.  What is Rock Music Doing to the Minds of our Youth? A First Experimental Look at the Effects of Rock Music Lyrics and Music Videos , 1987 .

[19]  P. Flowers Patterns of Attention in Music Listening , 2001 .

[20]  J. McCrary Effects of Listeners' and Performers' Race on Music Preferences , 1993 .

[21]  Albert LeBlanc,et al.  Effects of Style, Tempo, and Performing Medium on Children' s Music Preference , 1981 .

[22]  Richard A. Griggs,et al.  The effects of experience on performance in Wason’s selection task , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[23]  Carlos R. Abril Children'S Attitudes Toward Languages and Perceptions of Performers’ Social Status in the Context of Songs , 2002 .

[24]  Carlos R. Abril Learning outcomes of two approaches to multicultural music education , 2006 .

[25]  Janice N Killian Effect of Model Characteristics on Musical Preference of Junior High Students , 1990 .

[26]  H. Tajfel,et al.  The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior. , 2004 .

[27]  I. Wells Rock Music , 2022, The World of Bob Dylan.

[28]  C. Victor Fung,et al.  Musicians' and Nonmusicians' Preferences for World Musics: Relation to Musical Characteristics and Familiarity , 1996 .

[29]  V. Konečni Elusive Effects of Artists' ‘Messages’ , 1984 .

[30]  J Tudor-Hart,et al.  On the nature of prejudice. , 1961, The Eugenics review.

[31]  Carlos R. Abril Multicultural dimensions and their effect on children's responses to pop songs performed in various languages , 2005 .

[32]  Cheung Shing Yeh,et al.  Preference Responses and Use of Written Descriptors among Music and Nonmusic Majors in the United States, Hong Kong, and the People's Republic of China , 1999 .

[33]  Adrian C. North,et al.  The social psychology of music. , 1997 .

[34]  A. Halpern The organization of memory for familiar songs , 1984 .

[35]  P. Flowers,et al.  Self-Reported Distractions of Middle School Students in Listening to Music and Prose , 2005 .

[36]  C. Fung,et al.  Music Style Preferences of Young Students in Hong Kong. , 2000 .

[37]  Howard Giles,et al.  Understanding language attitudes: Exploring listener affect and identity , 1997 .