Picturing words? Sensorimotor cortex activation for printed words in child and adult readers

Highlights • We tested how picture-like responses to printed words develop in the child cortex.• Tool versus animal pictures and their names engaged similar brain areas in adults.• The 7–10 year-old sensorimotor cortex showed specialization for picture categories.• But names evoked no similar BOLD patterns despite good reading in older children.• So, automatic picturing of words’ sensorimotor meanings takes years to develop.

[1]  D. Plaut,et al.  Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. , 2000, Psychological review.

[2]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  P. Matthews,et al.  Category-related activation for written words in the posterior fusiform is task specific , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Treebank Penn,et al.  Linguistic Data Consortium , 1999 .

[5]  P. Downing,et al.  Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[7]  S E Shaywitz,et al.  Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. , 2001, Journal of communication disorders.

[8]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  Amygdala Responses to Fearful and Happy Facial Expressions under Conditions of Binocular Suppression , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Functional links between motor and language systems , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Scullin,et al.  Individual and developmental differences in suggestibility , 2004 .

[12]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[13]  Speak Louder,et al.  Actions speak louder. , 1979, Hospital progress.

[14]  Mark H. Johnson Interactive Specialization: A domain-general framework for human functional brain development? , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomical Differences Between Adults and School-Age Children in the Processing of Single Words , 2002, Science.

[16]  S. Petersen,et al.  A developmental fMRI study of reading and repetition reveals changes in phonological and visual mechanisms over age. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Development of neural systems for reading. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[19]  Richard R. Rosinski,et al.  Picture-word interference is semantically based. , 1977 .

[20]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[21]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Conceptual Flexibility in the Human Brain: Dynamic Recruitment of Semantic Maps from Visual, Motor, and Motion-related Areas , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  The causal role of category-specific neuronal representations in the left ventral premotor cortex (PMv) in semantic processing , 2010, NeuroImage.

[23]  E. Darcy Burgund,et al.  Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  F. Pulvermüller Semantic embodiment, disembodiment or misembodiment? In search of meaning in modules and neuron circuits , 2013, Brain and Language.

[25]  Xi-Nian Zuo,et al.  Resting-State Functional Connectivity Indexes Reading Competence in Children and Adults , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing , 2008, Brain and Language.

[27]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Embodied Language: A Review of the Role of the Motor System in Language Comprehension , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Do Words Really Interfere in Naming Pictures , 1976 .

[29]  H. Sakata,et al.  Selectivity for the shape, size, and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of monkey parietal area AIP. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. , 1975 .

[31]  S. Petersen,et al.  Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  J. Haxby,et al.  Parallel Visual Motion Processing Streams for Manipulable Objects and Human Movements , 2002, Neuron.

[33]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  The time course of action and action-word comprehension in the human brain as revealed by neurophysiology , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Two distinct neural mechanisms for category-selective responses. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Category-Specific Organization in the Human Brain Does Not Require Visual Experience , 2009, Neuron.

[36]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Do Children and the Elderly Show Heightened Semantic Priming? How to Answer the Question. , 1994 .

[37]  Claudio Mulatti,et al.  Picture–word interference and the response–exclusion hypothesis , 2012, Cortex.

[38]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[39]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Modeling parietal-premotor interactions in primate control of grasping , 1998, Neural Networks.

[40]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Detecting and adjusting for artifacts in fMRI time series data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[41]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[42]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Prediction of Individual Brain Maturity Using fMRI , 2010, Science.

[43]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data , 2001, NeuroImage.

[44]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  M. Kiefer,et al.  The Sound of Concepts: Four Markers for a Link between Auditory and Conceptual Brain Systems , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  J. Giedd,et al.  Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[47]  Christian Gerlach,et al.  A Review of Functional Imaging Studies on Category Specificity , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Stimulus properties matter more than perspective: An fMRI study of mental imagery and silent reading of action phrases , 2007, NeuroImage.

[49]  S. Small,et al.  TMS-induced modulation of action sentence priming in the ventral premotor cortex , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[51]  K. James,et al.  Auditory verb perception recruits motor systems in the developing brain: an fMRI investigation. , 2009, Developmental science.

[52]  S. Petersen,et al.  Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  Objects and their actions: evidence for a neurally distributed semantic system , 2003, NeuroImage.

[54]  H. Sakata,et al.  Deficit of hand preshaping after muscimol injection in monkey parietal cortex , 1994, Neuroreport.

[55]  Amir Amedi,et al.  Visual Cortex Extrastriate Body-Selective Area Activation in Congenitally Blind People “Seeing” by Using Sounds , 2014, Current Biology.

[56]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  The Feasibility of a Common Stereotactic Space for Children and Adults in fMRI Studies of Development , 2002, NeuroImage.

[57]  R. Poldrack Interpreting developmental changes in neuroimaging signals , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[58]  Xiaoping P. Hu,et al.  fMRI evidence for word association and situated simulation in conceptual processing , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[59]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Face perception: domain specific, not process specific. , 2004, Neuron.

[60]  JamesW. Lewis Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[61]  M. Arbib From grasp to language: Embodied concepts and the challenge of abstraction , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[62]  Kate Nation,et al.  Learning to Read Words , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[63]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[64]  Daniel Casasanto,et al.  Flexibility in Embodied Language Understanding , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[65]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category‐specific semantic information: the mismatch negativity to action words , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[66]  Martin I. Sereno,et al.  Dorsal and Ventral Stream Activation and Object Recognition Performance in School-age Children , 2010 .

[67]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Semantic activation in action planning. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  K. Nakayama,et al.  RESPONSE PROPERTIES OF THE HUMAN FUSIFORM FACE AREA , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[69]  Greg B. Simpson,et al.  Lexical ambiguity and children's word recognition. , 1986 .

[70]  M. Brett,et al.  Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  C. Gross,et al.  Spatial maps for the control of movement , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[72]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Nonvisual and Visual Object Shape Representations in Occipitotemporal Cortex: Evidence from Congenitally Blind and Sighted Adults , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[73]  Brian A. Wandell,et al.  The Development of Cortical Sensitivity to Visual Word Forms , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[74]  O. Muzik,et al.  Statistical Parametric Mapping: Assessment of Application in Children , 2000, NeuroImage.

[75]  Tom M. Mitchell,et al.  From the SelectedWorks of Marcel Adam Just 2011 Commonality of neural representations of words and pictures , 2016 .

[76]  Jody C. Culham,et al.  Does tool-related fMRI activity within the intraparietal sulcus reflect the plan to grasp? , 2007, NeuroImage.

[77]  J. Haxby,et al.  Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[78]  Greg B. Simpson,et al.  The Development of Automatic and Conscious Components of Contextual Facilitation. , 1983 .

[79]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[80]  Alison J. Clarke,et al.  “Actions Speak Louder” , 2013 .