Influence of different modes of real time visual information on single aimed movements.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Carlton. Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] D. Elliott,et al. Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .
[3] Robert L Sainburg,et al. Nondominant arm advantages in load compensation during rapid elbow joint movements. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] C Ghez,et al. Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] R. D Seidler,et al. Patterns of transfer of adaptation among body segments , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.
[6] P. Fitts,et al. INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[7] G. Stelmach,et al. Adaptation of handwriting size under distorted visual feedback in patients with Parkinson's disease and elderly and young controls , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[8] Digby Elliott,et al. Vision and motor control , 1992 .
[9] P. Bédard,et al. On the role of peripheral visual afferent information for the control of rapid video-aiming movements. , 2003, Acta psychologica.
[10] Robert R A van Doorn,et al. Effects of adaptation to altered display gain on the control of single aimed movements. , 2005, Motor control.
[11] Digby Elliott,et al. Rescaling an acquired discrete aiming movement: Specific or general motor learning? , 1997 .
[12] P J Keuss,et al. The role of vision in the temporal and spatial control of handwriting. , 1992, Acta psychologica.
[13] L. Proteau,et al. On the Role of Visual Afferent Information for the Control of Aiming Movements Toward Targets of Different Sizes , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[14] J. Saunders,et al. Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[15] Daniel W. Repperger,et al. Fitts' law and the microstructure of rapid discrete movements. , 1980 .
[16] Gerard P. van Galen,et al. On-line size control in handwriting demonstrates the continuous nature of motor programs , 1998 .
[17] S. Vogt,et al. Vision of the hand and environmental context in human prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[18] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[19] J. J. Bloomberg,et al. Context-dependent arm pointing adaptation , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.
[20] Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al. Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.
[21] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Contributions of vision and proprioception to accuracy in limb movements , 1994 .
[22] Romeo Chua,et al. Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .
[23] J. F. Soechting,et al. Effect of target size on spatial and temporal characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] M. A. Goodale,et al. The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[25] H. Zelaznik,et al. Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.
[26] P Viviani,et al. Perceptual anticipation in handwriting: The role of implicit motor competence , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] Laurette Hay,et al. Contribution of visual information to feedforward and feedback processes in rapid pointing movements , 1986 .
[28] R. Schmidt,et al. An Inverted-U Relation Between Spatial Error and Force Requirements in Rapid Limb Movements: Further Evidence for the Impulse-Variability Model , 1982 .
[29] H. Zelaznik,et al. Kinematics properties of rapid aimed hand movements. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.
[30] Y. Coello,et al. Retinal and extra-retinal contribution to position coding , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[31] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[32] C. Bard,et al. Role of peripheral vision in the directional control of rapid aiming movements , 1985 .
[33] R A Abrams,et al. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.
[34] Matthew Heath,et al. The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .
[35] Gavin P. Lawrence,et al. Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the control of movement amplitude. , 2003, Acta psychologica.
[36] James L. Lyons,et al. Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[37] R. J. van Beers,et al. The role of execution noise in movement variability. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[38] James Gordon,et al. Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[39] W. Helsen,et al. A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[40] Yann Coello,et al. Determination of target distance in a structured environment: Selection of visual information for action , 2000 .
[41] K. Boivin,et al. Exploring the Limits of Peripheral Vision for the Control of Movement , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[42] L. Proteau. Chapter 4 On The Specificity of Learning and the Role of Visual Information for Movement Control , 1992 .