Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets.

Attention can be used to keep track of moving items, particularly when there are multiple targets of interest that cannot all be followed with eye movements. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to investigate cortical regions involved in attentive tracking. Cortical flattening techniques facilitated within-subject comparisons of activation produced by attentive tracking, visual motion, discrete attention shifts, and eye movements. In the main task, subjects viewed a display of nine green "bouncing balls" and used attention to mentally track a subset of them while fixating. At the start of each attentive-tracking condition, several target balls (e.g., 3/9) turned red for 2 s and then reverted to green. Subjects then used attention to keep track of the previously indicated targets, which were otherwise indistinguishable from the nontargets. Attentive-tracking conditions alternated with passive viewing of the same display when no targets had been indicated. Subjects were pretested with an eye-movement monitor to ensure they could perform the task accurately while fixating. For seven subjects, functional activation was superimposed on each individual's cortically unfolded surface. Comparisons between attentive tracking and passive viewing revealed bilateral activation in parietal cortex (intraparietal sulcus, postcentral sulcus, superior parietal lobule, and precuneus), frontal cortex (frontal eye fields and precentral sulcus), and the MT complex (including motion-selective areas MT and MST). Attentional enhancement was absent in early visual areas and weak in the MT complex. However, in parietal and frontal areas, the signal change produced by the moving stimuli was more than doubled when items were tracked attentively. Comparisons between attentive tracking and attention shifting revealed essentially identical activation patterns that differed only in the magnitude of activation. This suggests that parietal cortex is involved not only in discrete shifts of attention between objects at different spatial locations but also in continuous "attentional pursuit" of moving objects. Attentive-tracking activation patterns were also similar, though not identical, to those produced by eye movements. Taken together, these results suggest that attentive tracking is mediated by a network of areas that includes parietal and frontal regions responsible for attention shifts and eye movements and the MT complex, thought to be responsible for motion perception. These results are consistent with theoretical models of attentive tracking as an attentional process that assigns spatial tags to targets and registers changes in their position, generating a high-level percept of apparent motion.

[1]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception , 1986, Vision Research.

[2]  A. Treisman,et al.  Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.

[3]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[4]  E A Cabanis,et al.  Location of the human posterior eye field with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[5]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  H. Helmholtz Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1963 .

[7]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Neuronal correlates of inferred motion in primate posterior parietal cortex , 1995, Nature.

[8]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[9]  S. Anstis The perception of apparent movement. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[10]  D. Levy,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Z. Pylyshyn The role of location indexes in spatial perception: A sketch of the FINST spatial-index model , 1989, Cognition.

[13]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.

[14]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional Analysis of V3A and Related Areas in Human Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Specific Involvement of Human Parietal Systems and the Amygdala in the Perception of Biological Motion , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Computerized Mappings of the Cerebral Cortex: A Multiresolution Flattening Method and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Top-down modulation of early sensory cortex. , 1997 .

[18]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Graded effects of spatial and featural attention on human area MT and associated motion processing areas. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[20]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  J V Haxby,et al.  Dissociation of saccade-related and pursuit-related activation in human frontal eye fields as revealed by fMRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Dawson,et al.  The how and why of what went where in apparent motion: modeling solutions to the motion correspondence problem. , 1991, Psychological review.

[23]  S. Yantis Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[24]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques , 1996, Nature.

[25]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Short-range vs long-range motion: not a valid distinction. , 1991, Spatial vision.

[26]  Z. Pylyshyn Some primitive mechanisms of spatial attention , 1994, Cognition.

[27]  A. Dale,et al.  New images from human visual cortex , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Functional Anatomy of a Prelearned Sequence of Horizontal Saccades in Humans , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Representation of motion boundaries in retinotopic human visual cortical areas , 1997, Nature.

[31]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Seeing Two as One: Linking Apparent Motion and Repetition Blindness , 1997 .

[32]  W. Schneider,et al.  Determining the locus of attentional selection with functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1996, NeuroImage.

[33]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 51–66 , 1909 .

[35]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  K Cheng,et al.  Human cortical regions activated by wide-field visual motion: an H2(15)O PET study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  M Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[39]  O J Braddick,et al.  Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[40]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[41]  R. Andersen,et al.  Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex , 1997, Nature.

[42]  A. Treisman Visual Attention and the Perception of Features and Objects , 1994 .

[43]  S. Miyauchi,et al.  Attention-regulated activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[46]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  T. Paus Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: A selective review , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  G. Orban,et al.  The kinetic occipital (KO) region in man: an fMRI study. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  A General Visual Attention Mechanism in the Human Brain: Evidence from fMRI , 1998, NeuroImage.

[50]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  R. Malach,et al.  Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  Bruce R. Rosen,et al.  Motion detection and correction in functional MR imaging , 1995 .

[53]  M Corbetta,et al.  Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. , 1990, Science.

[54]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visual working memory for shape and 3D‐orientation: a PET study , 1997, Neuroreport.

[55]  G. Orban,et al.  Many areas in the human brain respond to visual motion. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  Z W Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.

[57]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking Multiple Items Through Occlusion: Clues to Visual Objecthood , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Human functional anatomy of visually guided finger movements. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[59]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[60]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET , 1993, Nature.

[61]  R. Andersen,et al.  Evidence for the lateral intraparietal area as the parietal eye field , 1992, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[62]  A. Dale,et al.  Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, Nature.

[63]  O E Favreau,et al.  Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[64]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. , 1988, Science.

[65]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[66]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[67]  S Zeki,et al.  Going beyond the information given: the relation of illusory visual motion to brain activity , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[68]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[69]  A Berthoz,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomy of the Human Visual Fixation System , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[70]  G. Glover,et al.  Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[71]  A. Dale,et al.  Selective averaging of rapidly presented individual trials using fMRI , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[72]  A. Dale,et al.  Improved Localizadon of Cortical Activity by Combining EEG and MEG with MRI Cortical Surface Reconstruction: A Linear Approach , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[74]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion capture of luminance stimuli by equiluminous color gratings and by attentive tracking , 1994, Vision Research.

[75]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[76]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[77]  E. Cabanis,et al.  The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy and Mri , 1991 .