Reversing the sequence: reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias.

The aims of the research were to (a) compare the alcohol attentional bias (AAB) of social, hazardous, and harmful drinkers and (b) assess the effects of alcohol attention-control training on the AAB and alcohol consumption of hazardous and harmful drinkers. Participants were social drinkers (N=40), hazardous drinkers (N=89), and harmful drinkers (N=92). Paper-and-pencil measures were used to collect information about participants' socio-demographic characteristics, health status, motivational structure, drinking-related locus of control and situational self-confidence, readiness to change, affect, and alcohol consumption. Computerized classic, alcohol- and concerns-Stroop tests were administered. All participants were tested individually, with the order of tests counterbalanced across participants. After the baseline assessment, the hazardous and harmful drinkers were trained with the Alcohol Attention-Control Training Program (AACTP) for two and four sessions, respectively. Both samples completed a post-training assessment, and the harmful drinkers also completed 3-month follow-up. Results indicated that (a) the harmful drinkers had larger AAB than the hazardous and the social drinkers; (b) the attentional training reduced the hazardous and harmful drinkers' AAB; and (c) the harmful drinkers showed post-training reductions in alcohol consumption and improvements on the other drinking-related indices. The harmful drinkers' improvements were maintained at the 3-month follow-up.

[1]  S. Tiffany,et al.  Classical conditioning mechanisms in alcohol dependence. , 2001 .

[2]  M. Kindt,et al.  Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the stroop task. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  F. Crews,et al.  Neurotoxicity and neurocognitive impairments with alcohol and drug-use disorders: potential roles in addiction and recovery. , 2001, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[4]  W. Miles Cox,et al.  Motivational Predictors of Readiness to Change Chronic Substance Abuse , 2000 .

[5]  K. Berridge,et al.  Incentive-sensitization and addiction. , 2001, Addiction.

[6]  G. Yeates,et al.  Effects of alcohol cues on cognitive processing in heavy and light drinkers. , 1999, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[7]  Reinout W Wiers,et al.  Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. , 2007, Addiction.

[8]  S. Tiffany,et al.  A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. , 1990, Psychological review.

[9]  T. Peters,et al.  International handbook of alcohol dependence and problems. , 2001 .

[10]  K. Berridge,et al.  The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction , 1993, Brain Research Reviews.

[11]  S. Tiffany,et al.  A cognitive processing model of alcohol craving and compulsive alcohol use. , 2000, Addiction.

[12]  Mi-Sook Park,et al.  Brain substrates of craving to alcohol cues in subjects with alcohol use disorder. , 2007, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[13]  M. Goldman,et al.  Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: process and structure in the alcohol expectancy network. , 1998, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[14]  W. M. Cox,et al.  Motivation and the Goal Theory of Current Concerns , 2011 .

[15]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Discriminative stimuli that control instrumental tobacco-seeking by human smokers also command selective attention , 2003, Psychopharmacology.

[16]  M. Goldman,et al.  Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: experimental evidence for a mediational process. , 1993, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[17]  B. Jones,et al.  A flicker paradigm for inducing change blindness reveals alcohol and cannabis information processing biases in social users. , 2003, Addiction.

[18]  E. Pothos,et al.  Cognitive-motivational predictors of excessive drinkers’ success in changing , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[19]  J. Fadardi,et al.  Alcohol attentional bias: drinking salience or cognitive impairment? , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[20]  R. Wiers,et al.  Implicit cognitive processes in psychopathology: an introduction. , 2007, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[21]  Frank Ryan,et al.  Attentional bias and alcohol dependence: a controlled study using the modified stroop paradigm. , 2002, Addictive behaviors.

[22]  A. Damasio Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .

[23]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  M. R. Kristian,et al.  Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers' treatment outcome. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[25]  E. Klinger,et al.  A Motivational Model of Alcohol Use: Determinants of Use and Change , 2011 .

[26]  D. Chisholm,et al.  Integration of mental health care into primary care , 2000, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[27]  E. Klinger,et al.  Handbook of Motivational Counseling , 2003 .

[28]  C. Kornreich,et al.  Correlation between inhibition, working memory and delimited frontal area blood flow measure by 99mTc-Bicisate SPECT in alcohol-dependent patients. , 2001, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[29]  W. Miller,et al.  Motivational Interviewing with Problem Drinkers: II. The Drinker's Check-up as a Preventive Intervention , 1988, Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[30]  Marlatt Ga,et al.  Taxonomy of high-risk situations for alcohol relapse: evolution and development of a cognitive-behavioral model. , 1996 .

[31]  John W. Finney,et al.  Brief Interventions for Alcohol Problems , 2004, Alcohol research & health : the journal of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.

[32]  W. Miles Cox,et al.  A Motivational Model of Alcohol Use , 1988 .

[33]  Javad Salehi Fadardi,et al.  The addiction-stroop test: Theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  N. Heather,et al.  Predictive validity of the Readiness to Change Questionnaire. , 1993, Addiction.

[35]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  W. Cox,et al.  The effects of alcohol cue exposure on non-dependent drinkers' attentional bias for alcohol-related stimuli. , 2003, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[37]  N. Heather,et al.  Development of a short 'readiness to change' questionnaire for use in brief, opportunistic interventions among excessive drinkers. , 1992, British journal of addiction.

[38]  W. Miller,et al.  Brief interventions for alcohol problems: a review. , 1993, Addiction.

[39]  K. Mann,et al.  Effects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients: results of a controlled "Alcohol Stroop" study. , 1995, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[40]  D. Musher-Eizenman,et al.  An alcohol expectancy-challenge prevention program for at-risk college women. , 2003, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[41]  R. Wiers,et al.  Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers , 2002 .

[42]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[43]  T. Palfai,et al.  The search for new ways to change implicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy drinkers. , 2006, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[44]  Katherine Perryman,et al.  Alcohol Needs Assessment Research Project (ANARP). The 2004 National Alcohol Needs Assessment for England , 2005 .

[45]  K. Mann,et al.  Alcoholics’ selective processing of alcohol related words and cognitive performance on a Stroop task , 1994, European Psychiatry.

[46]  W. Miller,et al.  Motivational Interviewing with Problem Drinkers , 1983, Behavioural Psychotherapy.

[47]  M. Field,et al.  Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[48]  M. Knapp,et al.  Client Socio-Demographic and Service Receipt Inventory – European Version: development of an instrument for international research , 2000, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[49]  K. Berridge,et al.  The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization view. , 2000, Addiction.

[50]  H. Nordby,et al.  Alcoholics' selective attention to alcohol stimuli: automated processing? , 2000, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[51]  D. Baucom,et al.  Are waiting-list control groups needed in future marital therapy outcome research? , 2003 .

[52]  Helen Waters,et al.  A demonstration of attentional bias, using a novel dual task paradigm, towards clinically salient material in recovering alcohol abuse patients? , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[53]  W. Fleischhacker,et al.  Comparison of acamprosate and placebo in long-term treatment of alcohol dependence , 1996, The Lancet.

[54]  F. McKenna,et al.  The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task. , 2001, British journal of psychology.

[55]  A W Stacy,et al.  Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. , 1997, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[56]  Morse Rm,et al.  The definition of alcoholism. The Joint Committee of the National Council on Alcoholism and Drug Dependence and the American Society of Addiction Medicine to Study the Definition and Criteria for the Diagnosis of Alcoholism. , 1992 .

[57]  J. Fadardi Motivational and cognitive determinants of attentional bias for alcohol-related stimuli : implications for a new attentional-training intervention , 2003 .

[58]  C G McCusker,et al.  Cognitive biases and addiction: an evolution in theory and method. , 2001, Addiction.

[59]  E. Pothos,et al.  Cognitive bias for alcohol-related information in inferential processes. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[60]  W. M. Cox,et al.  Alcohol-related words are distracting to both alcohol abusers and non-abusers in the Stroop colour-naming task. , 1998, Addiction.

[61]  Matt Field,et al.  Experimental manipulation of attentional biases in heavy drinkers: do the effects generalise? , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[62]  J. Fadardi,et al.  Alcohol-attentional bias and motivational structure as independent predictors of social drinkers' alcohol consumption. , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[63]  J. S. Monahan Coloring Single Stroop Elements: Reducing Automaticity or Slowing Color Processing? , 2001, The Journal of general psychology.

[64]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[65]  M. Field,et al.  Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[66]  Reinout W Wiers,et al.  Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions. , 2002, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[67]  D. Besner,et al.  On a variant of Stroop’s paradigm: Which cognitions press your buttons? , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[68]  R. Morse,et al.  The Definition of Alcoholism , 1992 .

[69]  S. Stewart,et al.  Effects of short-term food deprivation and chronic dietary restraint on the selective processing of appetitive-related cues. , 1997, The International journal of eating disorders.

[70]  R. Wiers,et al.  An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers. , 2004, Addictive behaviors.

[71]  John J Curtin,et al.  Alcohol and cognitive control: implications for regulation of behavior during response conflict. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[72]  W. Hall,et al.  A randomised controlled trial of methadone maintenance treatment versus wait list control in an Australian prison system. , 2003, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[73]  J. Finney,et al.  Brief interventions for alcohol problems: a meta-analytic review of controlled investigations in treatment-seeking and non-treatment-seeking populations. , 2002, Addiction.

[74]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[75]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Alcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words. , 1994 .

[76]  G. Koob,et al.  The neurobiology of addiction: an overview. , 1997 .

[77]  P. Giancola,et al.  Executive cognitive functioning in alcohol use disorders. , 1998, Recent developments in alcoholism : an official publication of the American Medical Society on Alcoholism, the Research Society on Alcoholism, and the National Council on Alcoholism.