Catch-up saccades in head-unrestrained conditions reveal that saccade amplitude is corrected using an internal model of target movement.

This study analyzes how human participants combine saccadic and pursuit gaze movements when they track an oscillating target moving along a randomly oriented straight line with the head free to move. We found that to track the moving target appropriately, participants triggered more saccades with increasing target oscillation frequency to compensate for imperfect tracking gains. Our sinusoidal paradigm allowed us to show that saccade amplitude was better correlated with internal estimates of position and velocity error at saccade onset than with those parameters 100 ms before saccade onset as head-restrained studies have shown. An analysis of saccadic onset time revealed that most of the saccades were triggered when the target was accelerating. Finally, we found that most saccades were triggered when small position errors were combined with large velocity errors at saccade onset. This could explain why saccade amplitude was better correlated with velocity error than with position error. Therefore, our results indicate that the triggering mechanism of head-unrestrained catch-up saccades combines position and velocity error at saccade onset to program and correct saccade amplitude rather than using sensory information 100 ms before saccade onset.

[1]  G. Westheimer Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. , 1954, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[2]  N H Barmack,et al.  Modification of eye movements by instantaneous changes in the velocity of visual targets. , 1970, Vision research.

[3]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Target position and velocity: The stimuli for smooth pursuit eye movements , 1980, Vision Research.

[4]  R. Kettner,et al.  Predictive smooth pursuit of complex two-dimensional trajectories demonstrated by perturbation responses in monkeys , 1997, Vision Research.

[5]  R. Tomlinson,et al.  Combined eye-head gaze shifts in the primate. I. Metrics. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  S de Brouwer,et al.  Role of retinal slip in the prediction of target motion during smooth and saccadic pursuit. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Asynchrony between position and motion signals in the saccadic system. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[10]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .

[11]  Laurent Perrinet,et al.  Saccadic foveation of a moving visual target in the rhesus monkey. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Olivier White,et al.  Computation of gaze orientation under unrestrained head movements , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[13]  Emilio Bizzi,et al.  The coordination of eye and head movement during smooth pursuit , 1978, Brain Research.

[14]  R. Kettner,et al.  Predictive smooth pursuit of complex two-dimensional trajectories in monkey: component interactions , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  G. Barnes,et al.  Extraction of visual motion information for the control of eye and head movement during head-free pursuit , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Different responses to small visual errors during initiation and maintenance of smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Target motion direction influence on tracking performance and head tracking strategies in head-unrestrained conditions. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[18]  J. Cogan,et al.  FIVE TYPES OF EYE MOVEMENT IN THE HORIZONTAL MERIDIAN PLANE OF THE FIELD OF REGARD , 2004 .

[19]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  A. Opstal,et al.  Human eye-head coordination in two dimensions under different sensorimotor conditions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Direct evidence for a position input to the smooth pursuit system. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Missal,et al.  What triggers catch-up saccades during visual tracking? , 2002, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[23]  M. Missal,et al.  Quantitative analysis of catch-up saccades during sustained pursuit. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  D. Munoz,et al.  Gaze control in the cat: studies and modeling of the coupling between orienting eye and head movements in different behavioral tasks. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Interaction between smooth anticipation and saccades during ocular orientation in darkness. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  G. Barnes,et al.  Independent control of head and gaze movements during head‐free pursuit in humans , 1999, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  D. Sparks,et al.  Eye-head coordination during head-unrestrained gaze shifts in rhesus monkeys. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  J. Badler,et al.  Divergence between oculomotor and perceptual causality. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[29]  W. Becker,et al.  Human oblique saccades: Quantitative analysis of the relation between horizontal and vertical components , 1990, Vision Research.

[30]  D. Robinson,et al.  Eye movements evoked by cerebellar stimulation in the alert monkey. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Predictive Control of Eye Tracking Movements , 1962 .

[32]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Target acceleration can be extracted and represented within the predictive drive to ocular pursuit. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  D. Robinson,et al.  The upper limit of human smooth pursuit velocity , 1985, Vision Research.

[34]  Kathleen E Cullen,et al.  Time course of vestibuloocular reflex suppression during gaze shifts. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry,et al.  Kalman Filtering Naturally Accounts for Visually Guided and Predictive Smooth Pursuit Dynamics , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  G Blohm,et al.  Saccadic compensation for smooth eye and head movements during head-unrestrained two-dimensional tracking. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry,et al.  Saccades and pursuit: two outcomes of a single sensorimotor process , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  Richard J. Krauzlis,et al.  Population coding of movement dynamics by cerebellar Purkinje cells , 2000, Neuroreport.

[39]  R. Tomlinson,et al.  Combined eye-head gaze shifts in the primate. II. Interactions between saccades and the vestibuloocular reflex. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  G R Barnes,et al.  The influence of briefly presented randomized target motion on the extraretinal component of ocular pursuit. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Jérome Fleuriet,et al.  Saccadic Interception of a Moving Visual Target after a Spatiotemporal Perturbation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  L. Ritchie Effects of cerebellar lesions on saccadic eye movements. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Experimental study and modeling of vestibulo-ocular reflex modulation during large shifts of gaze in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  D Guitton,et al.  Human head-free gaze saccades to targets flashed before gaze-pursuit are spatially accurate. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.