The Impact of Menstrual Cycle Phase on Economic Choice and Rationality

It is well known that hormones affect both brain and behavior, but less is known about the extent to which hormones affect economic decision-making. Numerous studies demonstrate gender differences in attitudes to risk and loss in financial decision-making, often finding that women are more loss and risk averse than men. It is unclear what drives these effects and whether cyclically varying hormonal differences between men and women contribute to differences in economic preferences. We focus here on how economic rationality and preferences change as a function of menstrual cycle phase in women. We tested adherence to the Generalized Axiom of Revealed Preference (GARP), the standard test of economic rationality. If choices satisfy GARP then there exists a well-behaved utility function that the subject’s decisions maximize. We also examined whether risk attitudes and loss aversion change as a function of cycle phase. We found that, despite large fluctuations in hormone levels, women are as technically rational in their choice behavior as their male counterparts at all phases of the menstrual cycle. However, women are more likely to choose risky options that can lead to potential losses while ovulating; during ovulation women are less loss averse than men and therefore more economically rational than men in this regard. These findings may have market-level implications: ovulating women more effectively maximize expected value than do other groups.

[1]  C. Weinberg,et al.  Timing of sexual intercourse in relation to ovulation. Effects on the probability of conception, survival of the pregnancy, and sex of the baby. , 1996, The New England journal of medicine.

[2]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  Ovulation, Female Competition, and Product Choice: Hormonal Influences on Consumer Behavior , 2011 .

[3]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[4]  J. Simpson,et al.  Changes in women's mate preferences across the ovulatory cycle. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  B. Sherwin,et al.  Sex steroids and affect in the surgical menopause: a double-blind, cross-over study , 1985, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[6]  Daniel R. Burghart,et al.  An expected utility maximizer walks into a bar... , 2013, Journal of risk and uncertainty.

[7]  R. Thornhill,et al.  Menstrual cycle variation in women's preferences for the scent of symmetrical men , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  The Menstrual Cycle and Performance Feedback Alter Gender Differences in Competitive Choices , 2014, Journal of Labor Economics.

[9]  P. Samuelson A Note on the Pure Theory of Consumer's Behaviour , 1938 .

[10]  H. Varian Goodness of Fit for Revealed Preference Tests , 1994 .

[11]  The probability of conception on different days of the cycle with respect to ovulation: an overview , 1997, Advances in contraception : the official journal of the Society for the Advancement of Contraception.

[12]  H. de Wit,et al.  Acute effects of d-amphetamine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle in women , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[13]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Experimental Tests of the Endowment Effect and the Coase Theorem , 1990, Journal of Political Economy.

[14]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[15]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  Money, Status, and the Ovulatory Cycle , 2014 .

[16]  D. Perrett,et al.  Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: Further evidence , 2000 .

[17]  J. T. Archie Clinical Gynecologic Endocrinology and Infertility , 1974 .

[18]  David Silbersweig,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex activity related to emotional processing changes across the menstrual cycle. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Ifat Levy,et al.  Like cognitive function, decision making across the life span shows profound age-related changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  Charles A. Holt,et al.  Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects , 2002 .

[21]  Yan Chen,et al.  Sealed Bid Auctions with Ambiguity: Theory and Experiments , 2007, J. Econ. Theory.

[22]  H. Houthakker Revealed Preference and the Utility Function , 1950 .

[23]  Steven W Gangestad,et al.  Human oestrus , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Rachel T. A. Croson,et al.  Gender Differences in Preferences , 2009 .

[25]  Thomas Buser,et al.  The impact of the menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptives on competitiveness , 2012 .

[26]  G. Saad,et al.  Calories, beauty, and ovulation: The effects of the menstrual cycle on food and appearance-related consumption , 2012 .

[27]  Daniella J. Furman,et al.  Menstrual cycle phase modulates reward-related neural function in women , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  David A. Puts,et al.  Mating context and menstrual phase affect women's preferences for male voice pitch , 2005 .

[29]  C. C. Wolf,et al.  Variability in ratings of trustworthiness across the menstrual cycle , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[30]  William T. Harbaugh,et al.  Garp for kids: On the development of rational choice behavior , 2001 .

[31]  I. Merchenthaler,et al.  Evidence of the Colocalization of Estrogen Receptor-β mRNA and Estrogen Receptor-α Immunoreactivity in Neurons of the Rat Forebrain. , 1998, Endocrinology.

[32]  A. Tversky,et al.  Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty , 1992 .

[33]  Burkhard C. Schipper,et al.  Menstrual Cycle and Competitive Bidding , 2012, Games Econ. Behav..

[34]  I. Merchenthaler,et al.  Evidence for the colocalization of estrogen receptor-beta mRNA and estrogen receptor-alpha immunoreactivity in neurons of the rat forebrain. , 1998, Endocrinology.

[35]  S. Evans,et al.  The effects of smoked cocaine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle in women , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[36]  Muriel Niederle,et al.  Gender and Competition , 2011 .

[37]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  The Neural Basis of Loss Aversion in Decision-Making Under Risk , 2007, Science.

[38]  Catherine C. Eckel,et al.  Men, Women and Risk Aversion: Experimental Evidence , 2008 .

[39]  Frank A. Quinn,et al.  Establishment of detailed reference values for luteinizing hormone, follicle stimulating hormone, estradiol, and progesterone during different phases of the menstrual cycle on the Abbott ARCHITECT® analyzer , 2006, Clinical chemistry and laboratory medicine.

[40]  B. McEwen,et al.  Estrogen actions in the central nervous system. , 1999, Endocrine reviews.

[41]  Kristina M. Durante,et al.  Do Women Feel Worse to Look Their Best? Testing the Relationship Between Self-Esteem and Fertility Status Across the Menstrual Cycle , 2009, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[42]  J. Kable,et al.  Ventromedial Frontal Lobe Damage Disrupts Value Maximization in Humans , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  S. Afriat THE CONSTRUCTION OF UTILITY FUNCTIONS FROM EXPENDITURE DATA , 1967 .

[44]  S. Mani Progestin receptor subtypes in the brain: the known and the unknown. , 2008, Endocrinology.

[45]  Christine R. Harris Shifts in Masculinity Preferences Across the Menstrual Cycle: Still Not There , 2013 .

[46]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Thinking like a trader selectively reduces individuals' loss aversion , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  Harold,et al.  Estrogen Actions Throughout the Brain , 2002 .

[48]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Hormonal Cycle Modulates Arousal Circuitry in Women Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  K. Grammer,et al.  Male facial attractiveness: evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design , 2001 .