Spatial coincidence of intentional actions modulates an implicit visuomotor control.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] F. A. Miles,et al. Initial ocular following in humans: A response to first-order motion energy , 2005, Vision Research.
[2] Alexander M. Harner,et al. Task-dependent influences of attention on the activation of human primary visual cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[3] K. D. De Valois,et al. Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.
[4] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[5] Hiroaki Gomi,et al. Implicit Visuomotor Processing for Quick Online Reactions Is Robust against Aging , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[6] Szonya Durant,et al. Temporal dependence of local motion induced shifts in perceived position , 2004, Vision Research.
[7] H. Gomi. Implicit online corrections of reaching movements , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[8] Aya Takemura,et al. Neuronal responses in MST reflect the post-saccadic enhancement of short-latency ocular following responses , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] A P Batista,et al. Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.
[10] J F Kalaska,et al. Integration of predictive feedforward and sensory feedback signals for online control of visually guided movement. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[12] C. Prablanc,et al. Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.
[13] F. Miles,et al. Visual stabilization of the eyes in primates , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[14] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Spatiotemporal Tuning of Rapid Interactions between Visual-Motion Analysis and Reaching Movement , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] B. Day,et al. Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] J. Crawford,et al. Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] R. Andersen,et al. Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.
[18] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] T. Vilis,et al. Gaze-Centered Updating of Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Large-Field Visual Motion Directly Induces an Involuntary Rapid Manual Following Response , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] M. Goodale,et al. Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] V. S. Ramachandran,et al. Interaction between colour and motion in human vision , 1987, Nature.
[23] Selective activation of human cortical area V5A by a rotating visual stimulus in fMRI; implication of attentional mechanisms , 1998, Neuroreport.
[24] Aya Takemura,et al. MST neurons code for visual motion in space independent of pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] F. A. Miles,et al. Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] F A Miles,et al. Short latency ocular-following responses in man , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[27] David Whitney,et al. Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] H. Akaike,et al. Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle , 1973 .
[29] David Whitney,et al. The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.
[30] E. Brenner,et al. Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.
[31] F A Miles,et al. Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. II. Dependence on a prior saccadic eye movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.
[32] D. Heeger,et al. Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[33] Stefan Treue,et al. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.
[34] A. Treisman,et al. Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.
[35] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern , 1999, Nature.
[36] S. Shimojo,et al. Motion capture changes to induced motion at higher luminance contrasts, smaller eccentricities, and larger inducer sizes , 1993, Vision Research.
[37] Kenji Kawano,et al. Anticipatory Gain Modulation in Preparation for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[38] W Pieter Medendorp,et al. Behavioral reference frames for planning human reaching movements. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[39] David Whitney,et al. Visually guided reaching depends on motion area MT+. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[40] F A Miles,et al. Initial Ocular Following in Humans Depends Critically on the Fourier Components of the Motion Stimulus , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[41] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST , 1996, Nature.