Mortality salience enhances racial in-group bias in empathic neural responses to others' suffering

Behavioral research suggests that mortality salience (MS) leads to increased in-group identification and in-group favoritism in prosocial behavior. What remains unknown is whether and how MS influences brain activity that mediates emotional resonance with in-group and out-group members and is associated with in-group favoritism in helping behavior. The current work investigated MS effects on empathic neural responses to racial in-group and out-group members' suffering. Experiments 1 and 2 respectively recorded event related potentials (ERPs) and blood oxygen level dependent signals to pain/neutral expressions of Asian and Caucasian faces from Chinese adults who had been primed with MS or negative affect (NA). Experiment 1 found that an early frontal/central activity (P2) was more strongly modulated by pain vs. neutral expressions of Asian than Caucasian faces, but this effect was not affected by MS vs. NA priming. However, MS relative to NA priming enhanced racial in-group bias in long-latency neural response to pain expressions over the central/parietal regions (P3). Experiment 2 found that MS vs. NA priming increased racial in-group bias in empathic neural responses to pain expression in the anterior and mid-cingulate cortex. Our findings indicate that reminding mortality enhances brain activity that differentiates between racial in-group and out-group members' emotional states and suggest a neural basis of in-group favoritism under mortality threat.

[1]  Jeff Schimel,et al.  The Scrooge Effect: Evidence that Mortality Salience Increases Prosocial Attitudes and Behavior , 2002 .

[2]  P. Sessa,et al.  Taking one's time in feeling other-race pain: an event-related potential investigation on the time-course of cross-racial empathy. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[3]  Chase S. Sherwell,et al.  Intergroup relationships do not reduce racial bias in empathic neural responses to pain , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Shihui Han,et al.  Transient and sustained neural responses to death-related linguistic cues. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[5]  Yi Rao,et al.  Oxytocin receptor gene and racial ingroup bias in empathy-related brain activity , 2015, NeuroImage.

[6]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Existential neuroscience: self-esteem moderates neuronal responses to mortality-related stimuli. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[7]  Béatrice de Gelder,et al.  Lateralization for dynamic facial expressions in human superior temporal sulcus , 2015, NeuroImage.

[8]  Simona Sacchi,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Castano Et Al. / Mortality Salience and Ingroup Entitativity I Belong Therefore I Exist: Ingroup Identification, Ingroup Entitativity, and Ingroup Bias , 2022 .

[9]  Charles D. Smith,et al.  Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[10]  D. Boussaoud,et al.  Spatial attention and memory versus motor preparation: premotor cortex involvement as revealed by fMRI. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  P. Roland,et al.  Fields in human motor areas involved in preparation for reaching, actual reaching, and visuomotor learning: a positron emission tomography study , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  Shihui Han,et al.  Neurocognitive processes of linguistic cues related to death , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  K. Preuschoff,et al.  Neural Responses to Ingroup and Outgroup Members' Suffering Predict Individual Differences in Costly Helping , 2010, Neuron.

[14]  F. D. de Waal Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[15]  Mark H. Davis The effects of dispositional empathy on emotional reactions and helping: A multidimensional approach , 1983 .

[16]  Shihui Han,et al.  Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: An event-related brain potential study , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  G. Rousselet,et al.  Neural repetition suppression to identity is abolished by other-race faces , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Jiangqun Liao,et al.  Who Uses Groups to Transcend the Limits of the Individual Self? Exploring the Effects of Interdependent Self-Construal and Mortality Salience on Investment in Social Groups , 2013 .

[19]  Claus Lamm,et al.  Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain , 2011, NeuroImage.

[20]  Shihui Han,et al.  Shared beliefs enhance shared feelings: Religious/irreligious identifications modulate empathic neural responses , 2014, Social neuroscience.

[21]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion affects action: Midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  Wen Zhou,et al.  Oxytocin modulates the racial bias in neural responses to others’ suffering , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[23]  M. Hogg,et al.  Mortality salience, self-esteem, and defense of the group: mediating role of in-group identification , 2015 .

[24]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[25]  Emiliano Macaluso,et al.  Their pain is not our pain: Brain and autonomic correlates of empathic resonance with the pain of same and different race individuals , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[26]  S. Preston,et al.  Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[27]  Sandra E. Ward,et al.  Reducing racial disparities in pain treatment: The role of empathy and perspective-taking , 2011, PAIN.

[28]  Shihui Han,et al.  Neural responses to perceived pain in others predict real-life monetary donations in different socioeconomic contexts , 2011, NeuroImage.

[29]  James D. Johnson,et al.  Rodney King and O. J. Revisited: The Impact of Race and Defendant Empathy Induction on Judicial Decisions , 2002 .

[30]  T. Valentine,et al.  Towards an Exemplar Model of Face Processing: The Effects of Race and Distinctiveness , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[31]  Lihua Mao,et al.  Gender difference in empathy for pain: An electrophysiological investigation , 2008, Brain Research.

[32]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Racial Bias Reduces Empathic Sensorimotor Resonance with Other-Race Pain , 2010, Current Biology.

[33]  Richard J. Harris,et al.  Morphing between expressions dissociates continuous from categorical representations of facial expression in the human brain , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  D. O. Sears,et al.  Prejudice and Politics: Symbolic Racism Versus Racial Threats to the Good Life , 1981 .

[35]  Shihui Han,et al.  Reminders of mortality decrease midcingulate activity in response to others' suffering. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[36]  Xiaoying Wang,et al.  Do You Feel My Pain? Racial Group Membership Modulates Empathic Neural Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Greenberg,et al.  The Causes and Consequences of a Need for Self-Esteem: A Terror Management Theory , 1986 .

[38]  C. Daniel Batson,et al.  Altruism in Humans , 2011 .

[39]  Niall W. Duncan,et al.  Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[40]  Tokiko Harada,et al.  Neural basis of extraordinary empathy and altruistic motivation , 2010, NeuroImage.

[41]  Qiang Liu,et al.  Task modulations of racial bias in neural responses to others' suffering , 2014, NeuroImage.

[42]  J. Greenberg,et al.  A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: an extension of terror management theory. , 1999, Psychological review.

[43]  D. Stapel,et al.  Go with the flow: conforming to others in the face of existential threat , 2008 .

[44]  F. Sani,et al.  Why Does Ingroup Identification Shield People from Death Anxiety , 2013 .

[45]  D. Templer,et al.  The measurement of death depression. , 1990, Journal of clinical psychology.

[46]  R. T. Pivik,et al.  Guidelines for the recording and quantitative analysis of electroencephalographic activity in research contexts. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[47]  K. Zilles,et al.  Neural activity in human primary motor cortex areas 4a and 4p is modulated differentially by attention to action. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Feng Sheng,et al.  Manipulations of cognitive strategies and intergroup relationships reduce the racial bias in empathic neural responses , 2012, NeuroImage.

[49]  M. Hallett,et al.  Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  C. Batson,et al.  Distress and empathy: two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. , 1987, Journal of personality.

[51]  Linda Simon,et al.  The effects of mortality salience on intergroup bias between minimal groups , 1996 .