Brain Mechanisms of Arithmetic: A Crucial Role for Ventral Temporal Cortex

Elementary arithmetic requires a complex interplay between several brain regions. The classical view, arising from fMRI, is that the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the superior parietal lobe (SPL) are the main hubs for arithmetic calculations. However, recent studies using intracranial electroencephalography have discovered a specific site, within the posterior inferior temporal cortex (pITG), that activates during visual perception of numerals, with widespread adjacent responses when numerals are used in calculation. Here, we reexamined the contribution of the IPS, SPL, and pITG to arithmetic by recording intracranial electroencephalography signals while participants solved addition problems. Behavioral results showed a classical problem size effect: RTs increased with the size of the operands. We then examined how high-frequency broadband (HFB) activity is modulated by problem size. As expected from previous fMRI findings, we showed that the total HFB activity in IPS and SPL sites increased with problem size. More surprisingly, pITG sites showed an initial burst of HFB activity that decreased as the operands got larger, yet with a constant integral over the whole trial, thus making these signals invisible to slow fMRI. Although parietal sites appear to have a more sustained function in arithmetic computations, the pITG may have a role of early identification of the problem difficulty, beyond merely digit recognition. Our results ask for a reevaluation of the current models of numerical cognition and reveal that the ventral temporal cortex contains regions specifically engaged in mathematical processing.

[1]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The functional architecture of the ventral temporal cortex and its role in categorization , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  C. Semenza,et al.  Balancing the 2 Hemispheres in Simple Calculation: Evidence From Direct Cortical Electrostimulation , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Finger Tracking Reveals the Covert Stages of Mental Arithmetic , 2017, Open Mind.

[4]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Effects of problem size and arithmetic operation on brain activation during calculation in children with varying levels of arithmetical fluency , 2011, NeuroImage.

[5]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: a brain imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  Comparing Parietal Quantity-Processing Mechanisms between Humans and Macaques , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  M. Ashcraft Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.

[8]  Alice De Visscher,et al.  The interference effect in arithmetic fact solving: An fMRI study , 2015, NeuroImage.

[9]  Jeremy R. Manning,et al.  Broadband Shifts in Local Field Potential Power Spectra Are Correlated with Single-Neuron Spiking in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Josef Parvizi,et al.  Electrocorticography reveals the temporal dynamics of posterior parietal cortical activity during recognition memory decisions , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Electrical Stimulation of Human Fusiform Face-Selective Regions Distorts Face Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Shachar Maidenbaum,et al.  A number-form area in the blind , 2015, Nature Communications.

[13]  J. Grafman,et al.  The calculating brain: an fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[15]  Carlo Semenza,et al.  Right parietal cortex and calculation processing: intraoperative functional mapping of multiplication and addition in patients affected by a brain tumor. , 2013, Journal of neurosurgery.

[16]  S. Dehaene Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .

[17]  Mareike Grotheer,et al.  A preference for mathematical processing outweighs the selectivity for Arabic numbers in the inferior temporal gyrus , 2018, NeuroImage.

[18]  Marinella Cappelletti The Neuropsychology of Acquired Number and Calculation Disorders , 2015 .

[19]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Sensory-motor mechanisms in human parietal cortex underlie arbitrary visual decisions , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal , 2001, Nature.

[22]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Origins of the brain networks for advanced mathematics in expert mathematicians , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  B. P. Klein,et al.  Topographic Representation of Numerosity in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2013, Science.

[24]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words , 1992, Cognition.

[25]  E. Spelke,et al.  Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.

[26]  G. Groen,et al.  A chronometric analysis of simple addition. , 1972 .

[27]  Amir Amedi,et al.  Origins of the specialization for letters and numbers in ventral occipitotemporal cortex , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[29]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociating Prefrontal and Parietal Cortex Activation during Arithmetic Processing , 2000, NeuroImage.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional and Structural Alterations of the Intraparietal Sulcus in a Developmental Dyscalculia of Genetic Origin , 2003, Neuron.

[31]  Pierre Barrouillet,et al.  On the problem-size effect in small additions: Can we really discard any counting-based account? , 2013, Cognition.

[32]  D. LeBihan,et al.  Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[33]  B. Foster,et al.  Intrinsic and Task-Dependent Coupling of Neuronal Population Activity in Human Parietal Cortex , 2015, Neuron.

[34]  Pierre Barrouillet,et al.  Fast automated counting procedures in addition problem solving: When are they used and why are they mistaken for retrieval? , 2016, Cognition.

[35]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  A Brain Area for Visual Numerals , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[37]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. , 2005 .

[38]  Amy L. Daitch,et al.  Mapping human temporal and parietal neuronal population activity and functional coupling during mathematical cognition , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Josef Parvizi,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses in the Ventral Temporal Cortex During Reading of Numerals and Calculation , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  Nick F. Ramsey,et al.  Automated electrocorticographic electrode localization on individually rendered brain surfaces , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[42]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[44]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Absence of visual experience modifies the neural basis of numerical thinking , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[46]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Different Origins of Gamma Rhythm and High-Gamma Activity in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2011, PLoS biology.

[47]  V. Michel,et al.  Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic , 2009, Science.