Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways

Fast, parallel word recognition, in expert readers, relies on sectors of the left ventral occipito-temporal pathway collectively known as the visual word form area. This expertise is thought to arise from perceptual learning mechanisms that extract informative features from the input strings. The perceptual expertise hypothesis leads to two predictions: (1) parallel word recognition, based on the ventral visual system, should be limited to words displayed in a familiar format (foveal horizontal words with normally spaced letters); (2) words displayed in formats outside this field of expertise should be read serially, under supervision of dorsal parietal attention systems. We presented adult readers with words that were progressively degraded in three different ways (word rotation, letter spacing, and displacement to the visual periphery). Behaviorally, we identified degradation thresholds above which reading difficulty increased non-linearly, with the concomitant emergence of a word length effect on reading latencies reflecting serial reading strategies. fMRI activations were correlated with reading difficulty in bilateral occipito-temporal and parietal regions, reflecting the strategies required to identify degraded words. A core region of the intraparietal cortex was engaged in all modes of degradation. Furthermore, in the ventral pathway, word degradation led to an amplification of activation in the posterior visual word form area, at a level thought to encode single letters. We also found an effect of word length restricted to highly degraded words in bilateral occipitoparietal regions. Those results clarify when and how the ventral parallel visual word form system needs to be supplemented by the deployment of dorsal serial reading strategies.

[1]  Daniel N. Bub,et al.  Different modes of word recognition in the left and right visual fields , 1988, Brain and Language.

[2]  Charles E Connor,et al.  Underlying principles of visual shape selectivity in posterior inferotemporal cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  Stéphane Lehéricy,et al.  The pathophysiology of letter-by-letter reading , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  Visual areas and spatial summation in human visual cortex , 2001, Vision Research.

[6]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Parietal cortex and attention , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  T. Poggio,et al.  Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[9]  B. Weekes Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency , 1997 .

[10]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for case-specific reading: the role of multi-letter units in word recognition. , 2001 .

[13]  Y. Yamane,et al.  Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  R. Salmelin,et al.  Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  M. Sereno,et al.  Mapping of Contralateral Space in Retinotopic Coordinates by a Parietal Cortical Area in Humans , 2001, Science.

[17]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Word Length and Orthographic Neighborhood Size Effects in the Left and Right Cerebral Hemispheres , 2002, Brain and Language.

[18]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[19]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Attention: From Modulation of Sensory Processing to Top-Down Control , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  B. Ans,et al.  Polysyllabic pseudo-word processing in reading and lexical decision: Converging evidence from behavioral data, connectionist simulations and functional MRI , 2006, Brain Research.

[21]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[22]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The Special Effect of Case Mixing on Word Identification: Neuropsychological and Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Studies Dissociating Case Mixing from Contrast Reduction , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Jean-Francois Mangin,et al.  Automatized clustering and functional geometry of human parietofrontal networks for language, space, and number , 2004, NeuroImage.

[24]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  Mariano Sigman,et al.  Parsing a sequence of brain activations at psychological times using fMRI , 2007, NeuroImage.

[27]  Olaf B. Paulson,et al.  Brain Activation during Word Identification and Word Recognition , 1998, NeuroImage.

[28]  Ron Borowsky,et al.  FMRI of Ventral and Dorsal Processing Streams in Basic Reading Processes: Insular Sensitivity to Phonology , 2006, Brain Topography.

[29]  J. Gottlieb From Thought to Action: The Parietal Cortex as a Bridge between Perception, Action, and Cognition , 2007, Neuron.

[30]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[31]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Neuropsychological Evidence for Case-Specific Reading: Multi-Letter Units in Visual Word Recognition , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[32]  J B Poline,et al.  Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.

[33]  Michal Lavidor,et al.  Why word length only matters in the left visual field , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Ilhan Raman,et al.  Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency: revisited. , 2008 .

[35]  E. Warrington Word-form dyslexia , 1995 .

[36]  Michal Lavidor An Examination of the Lateralized Abstractive/Form Specific Model Using MiXeD-CaSe Primes , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[37]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual attention: Insights from brain imaging , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Human cortical mechanisms of visual attention during orienting and search. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[39]  Thomas A Zeffiro,et al.  Development of neural mechanisms for reading , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  K. Pammer,et al.  Attentional shifting and the role of the dorsal pathway in visual word recognition , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[42]  S. Ullman Object recognition and segmentation by a fragment-based hierarchy , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  M. Sigman,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .

[44]  Lawrence M. Parsons,et al.  Superior parietal cortices and varieties of mental rotation , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network , 2004, NeuroImage.

[46]  B. Ans,et al.  Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies , 2003 .

[47]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[48]  Valérie Hahn-Barma,et al.  What and Where in Word Reading: Ventral Coding of Written Words Revealed by Parietal Atrophy , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  R. Dolan,et al.  Switching between the Forest and the Trees: Brain Systems Involved in Local/Global Changed-Level Judgments , 2001, NeuroImage.

[50]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading—XVI. The visual span in normal and low vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[51]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.

[52]  C. J. Price,et al.  The Effect of Varying Stimulus Rate and Duration on Brain Activity during Reading , 1996, NeuroImage.

[53]  E. Rolls,et al.  View-invariant representations of familiar objects by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  M Lavidor,et al.  Evaluating a split processing model of visual word recognition: effects of word length. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[55]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Brain activations during letter-by-letter reading: A follow-up study , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  Glyn Humphreys,et al.  A selective effect of parietal damage on letter identification in mixed case words , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional Analysis of V3A and Related Areas in Human Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Differential effects of word length and visual contrast in the fusiform and lingual gyri during , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Psychophysical and physiological evidence for viewer-centered object representations in the primate. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  K. Amunts,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex is implicated in continuous switching between verbal fluency tasks: an fMRI study with clinical implications , 2002 .

[62]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Topographical Layout of Hand, Eye, Calculation, and Language-Related Areas in the Human Parietal Lobe , 2002, Neuron.

[63]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.

[64]  J. Norman,et al.  Why is word recognition impaired by disorientation while the identification of single letters is not? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The temporal dynamics of reading: a PET study , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[66]  B. Ans,et al.  A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. , 1998, Psychological review.

[67]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[68]  Jens Schwarzbach,et al.  Control of object-based attention in human cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[69]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[70]  T. Nazir,et al.  Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words , 2004, Brain and Language.

[71]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[72]  T. Nazir,et al.  Developing normal reading skills: aspects of the visual processes underlying word recognition. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[73]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Direct Intracranial, fMRI, and Lesion Evidence for the Causal Role of Left Inferotemporal Cortex in Reading , 2006, Neuron.

[74]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Functions of the Primate Temporal Lobe Cortical Visual Areas in Invariant Visual Object and Face Recognition , 2000, Neuron.

[76]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during imagined spatial transformations , 2006, NeuroImage.

[77]  J. Norman,et al.  Reading rotated words. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[78]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Length, formats, neighbours, hemispheres, and the processing of words presented laterally or at fixation , 2004, Brain and Language.

[79]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[80]  Michal Lavidor,et al.  Analysis of standard and non-standard visual word format in the two hemispheres , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The Effects of Case Mixing on Word Recognition: Evidence from a PET Study , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.