First-Trial "Adaptation" to Prism Exposure: Artifact of Visual Capture
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] B Wallace,et al. Prism Exposure Aftereffects and Direct Effects for Different Movement and Feedback Times , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[2] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Adaptive Spatial Alignment , 1997 .
[3] C. S. Harris,et al. Beware of the Straight-Ahead Shift—A Nonperceptual Change in Experiments on Adaptation to Displaced Vision , 1974, Perception.
[4] S. Kitazawa,et al. Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] Dottie M. Clower,et al. Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[6] B Wallace,et al. Effects on prism adaptation of duration and timing of visual feedback during pointing. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[7] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. The rate of adaptation to displacement prisms remains constant despite acquisition of rapid calibration , 1999 .
[8] Joel Goldberg,et al. Immediate correction and adaptation based on viewing a prismatically displaced scene , 1966 .
[9] B Wallace,et al. Calibration and Alignment are Separable: Evidence From Prism Adaptation , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[10] G M Redding,et al. Additivity in adaptation to optical tilt. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] I. Kohler,et al. The formation and transformation of the perceptual world. , 1963 .
[12] L. K. Canon,et al. Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[13] B Wallace,et al. Prism Adaptation During Target Pointing From Visible and Nonvisible Starting Locations. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.
[14] Steven M. Finbeiner,et al. The Neural and Behavioral Organization of Goal-Directed Movements , 1989, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[15] G. M. Redding,et al. Effects of Pointing Rate and Availability of Visual Feedback on Visual and Proprioceptive Components of Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.
[16] G. M. Redding,et al. Perceptual-motor coordination and prism adaptation during locomotion: A control for head posture contributions , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[17] C. Hall-Haro,et al. Learning motor synergies makes use of information on muscular load. , 2000, Learning & memory.
[18] D. H. Warren,et al. Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.
[19] R. Welch. Perceptual modification : adapting to altered sensory environments / Robert B. Welch , 1978 .
[20] Benjamin Wallace,et al. Dual Prism Adaptation: Calibration or Alignment? , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[21] Benjamin Wallace,et al. First-Trial Adaptation to Prism Exposure , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[22] B Craske,et al. No retinal component in prism adaptation. , 1974, Acta psychologica.
[23] J. Smart. Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified science of the mind-brain Patricia Smith Churchland. Bradford Books, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts (1986). xiv + 546 pp. , 1987, Neuroscience.
[24] H. Pick,et al. Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .
[25] B Wallace,et al. Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] R B Welch,et al. An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] M. Hagen,et al. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. , 2002, The American psychologist.
[28] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] B Wallace,et al. Effects of movement duration and visual feedback on visual and proprioceptive components of prism adaptation. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.
[30] M. Jeannerod. The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.
[31] G. M. Redding,et al. Head posture effects in prism adaptation during hallway exposure , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[32] The dissection of intersensory bias: weighting for Radeau , 1994, Current psychology of cognition = Cahiers de psychologie cognitive : CPC.
[33] W. Helsen,et al. A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[34] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Perceptual distortion contributes to the curvature of human reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] B Wallace,et al. Adaptive mechanisms in perceptual-motor coordination: components of prism adaptation. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.
[36] B Wallace,et al. Phenomenal versus process explanations of prism aftereffects. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.
[37] J. Tastevin,et al. En partant de l'expérience d'Aristote les déplacements artificiels des parties du corps ne sont pas suivis par le sentiment de ces parties ni par les sensations qu'on peut y produire. , 1937 .
[38] G. M. Redding,et al. Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] B Wallace,et al. Adaptive coordination and alignment of eye and hand. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[40] G. M. Redding,et al. Strategie Calibration and Spatial Alignment: A Model From Prism Adaptation , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.