The belief-based emotion of surprise: the case for a lag in understanding relative to false belief

Three experiments were carried out to determine whether there is a lag in predicting surprise relative to false belief. All 3 experiments used "backwards reasoning" tasks. The findings were that ( a ) there is a lag in predicting surprise relative to false belief, ( b ) by 5 or 6 years of age children claim that one will be surprised when they gain knowledge of that which they were previously ignorant or when they discover that they had previously held a false belief, ( c ) by 7 to 9 years of age they understand that surprise will more likely result from false beliefs rather than mere ignorance, and ( d ) children's difficulty understanding surprise as specifically belief-based does not likely stem from information processing limitations. It is argued that the lag likely results because children must build a new concept of surprise (e.g., from desire- to belief-based).

[1]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Young children's reasoning about beliefs , 1988, Cognition.

[2]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[3]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988, Child development.

[4]  William Kessen,et al.  History, theory, and methods , 1983 .

[5]  J. Flavell,et al.  Inferring false beliefs from actions and reactions. , 1990, Child development.

[6]  C. Izard The face of emotion , 1971 .

[7]  C. Lewis,et al.  Three-year-olds' problems with false belief: conceptual deficit or linguistic artifact? , 1990, Child development.

[8]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Against the Cartesian view on mind: Young children's difficulty with own false beliefs , 1991 .

[9]  M. Doherty,et al.  Preschoolers' grasp of a desire for knowledge in false‐belief prediction: Practical intelligence and verbal report , 1991 .

[10]  David R. Olson,et al.  Trick or treat: Children's understanding of surprise , 1993 .

[11]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[12]  H. Wellman,et al.  From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology , 1990, Cognition.

[13]  H. Borke,et al.  Interpersonal Perception of Young Children: Egocentrism or Empathy?. , 1971 .

[14]  J. Russell,et al.  Further Evidence on Preschoolers' Interpretation of Facial Expressions , 1985 .

[15]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Young Children's Understanding of Changes in Their Mental States. , 1991 .

[16]  H. Wellman,et al.  Mind and emotion: Children's understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires , 1991 .

[17]  D. Zaitchik Is only seeing really believing?: Sources of the true belief in the false belief task , 1991 .

[18]  Josef Perner,et al.  Pleased and surprised: Children's cognitive theory of emotion , 1991 .

[19]  D. Olson,et al.  The ABCs of Deception: Do Young Children Understand Deception in the Same Way as Adults?. , 1993 .

[20]  H. Wellman The Child's Theory of Mind , 1990 .

[21]  M. Lewis,et al.  What Do Children Know about Emotions and When Do They Know It , 1985 .

[22]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[23]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[24]  Nicola Yuill,et al.  Young children's coordination of motive and outcome in judgements of satisfaction and morality , 1984 .

[25]  M. Siegal,et al.  Where to look first for children's knowledge of false beliefs , 1991, Cognition.

[26]  P. Ekman The face of man , 1980 .