The locus of interference in the Simon effect: an ERP study

The Simon effect was studied using ERPs and the lateralized readiness potential (LRP). In Experiment 1, visual stimuli requiring left or right hand responses were presented at either side of fixation. In Experiment 2, the empty side was occupied by a distractor. Spatial S-R incompatibility delayed P300 latency in both experiments, as predicted by perceptual interference accounts of the Simon effect. However, the LRP results were not conclusive. The lateralized presentation of visual stimuli (Experiment 1) or targets (Experiment 2) produced a posterior ERP hemispheric asymmetry that spread to central electrodes and obscured the early development of the LRP. Experiment 3 avoided the problem by presenting the stimuli in the vertical meridian, and the LRP showed signs of incorrect response preparation in incompatible trials, in agreement with response-interference models. Both perceptual and response interference may be present in the Simon effect.

[1]  P. Barber,et al.  Interference effects in the Stroop and Simon paradigms. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  R. Proctor,et al.  Processing of an irrelevant location dimension as a function of the relevant stimulus dimension. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  A. Milner,et al.  Visual evoked potentials to lateralized visual stimuli and the measurement of interhemispheric transmission time , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  E. Donchin,et al.  Detecting early communication: Using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[8]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Effect of irrelevant information on the processing of relevant information: Facilitation and/or interference? The influence of experimental design , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  J. Richard Simon,et al.  The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on choice reaction time: Duration of the phenomenon and its relation to stages of processing , 1976 .

[10]  M G Coles,et al.  On the transmission of partial information: inferences from movement-related brain potentials. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  J R Simon,et al.  Processing symbolic information from a visual display: interference from an irrelevant directional cue. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  H. Leuthold,et al.  Covert effects of alcohol revealed by event-related potentials , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  W. B. Matthews Motivation, motor and sensory processes of the brain: electrical potentials, behaviour and clinical use. Proceedings of the 5th International Symposium Ulm-Reisensburg. May 14-18, 1979. , 1982, Progress in brain research.

[14]  S. Kornblum Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility. , 1992 .

[15]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[16]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. , 1995 .

[17]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[18]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[19]  S. Kornblum,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Bernard Renault,et al.  Latencies of event related potentials as a tool for studying motor processing organization , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[21]  A. Hedge,et al.  The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. , 1975, Acta psychologica.

[22]  J R Simon,et al.  Effects of an irrelevant auditory stimulus on visual choice reaction time. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[24]  G Mulder,et al.  The continuous flow model revisited: perceptual and central motor aspects. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[25]  B. Hommel Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.

[26]  G Mulder,et al.  Event-related potentials during memory search and selective attention to letter size and conjunctions of letter size and color. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[27]  G Mulder,et al.  Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: a psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[28]  D. Jeffreys,et al.  Source locations of pattern-specific components of human visual evoked potentials. I. Component of striate cortical origin , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  T Hasbroucq,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: toward a conceptual clarification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  R. Ragot,et al.  Mental processing during reactions toward and away from a stimulus: an ERP analysis of auditory congruence and S-R compatibility. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[31]  A Rémond,et al.  Hemispheric involvement during a bimanual RT task: P300 and motor potential. , 1980, Progress in brain research.

[32]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  An additive factors analysis of the effect(s) of location cues associated with auditory stimuli on stages of information processing. , 1989, Acta psychologica.

[33]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[34]  E Donchin,et al.  A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.

[35]  G Mulder,et al.  Visual spatial attention to stimuli presented on the vertical and horizontal meridian: an ERP study. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[36]  L. Mulder,et al.  Use of partial stimulus information in response processing. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  H G Vaughan,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of two stages of information processing in physical and semantic discrimination tasks. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[38]  R. Ragot,et al.  Perceptual and motor space representation: an event-related potential study. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[39]  R. Ragot Cerebral Evoked Potentials: Early Indexes of Compatibility Effects , 1990 .

[40]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[41]  Richard Ragot,et al.  P300, as a function of S—R compatibility and motor programming , 1981, Biological Psychology.