Elaboration enhances the imagined contact effect

Recent studies have shown that imagining intergroup contact can improve attitudes held toward a range of social groups. We extended research on imagined contact by testing an elaborated task variant designed specifically to enhance future contact intentions. In three experiments imagined contact heightened intentions, elaborated imagery enhanced this effect, and these enhancements were attributable to both reduced intergroup anxiety and an increase in the reported vividness of the imagined scenario. Furthermore, prior contact enhanced the vividness with which imagined scenarios were envisaged, with concurrent benefits for future contact intentions. Results also supported the notion that elaboration creates a more accessible contact script upon which to base future judgments of intention. We discuss the implications of these findings for a developing model of imagined contact effects.

[1]  Thomas L. Webb,et al.  Identifying good opportunities to act: Implementation intentions and cue discrimination , 2004 .

[2]  W. Brewer Remembering reconsidered: Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical events , 1988 .

[3]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[4]  Lindsey Cameron,et al.  Changing Children’s Intergroup Attitudes Toward Refugees: Testing Different Models of Extended Contact , 2022 .

[5]  Peter W. Sheehan,et al.  The function and nature of imagery , 1972 .

[6]  C. Anderson Imagination and expectation: The effect of imagining behavioral scripts on personal influences. , 1983 .

[7]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Data analysis in social psychology. , 1998 .

[8]  B. Latané,et al.  Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  S. C. Ouellette Notes for a Critical Personality Psychology: Making Room under the Critical Psychology Umbrella , 2008 .

[10]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures , 1978 .

[11]  C. G. Lord,et al.  Effects of Directed Thinking on Intentions to Engage in Beneficial Activities: Idea Generation or Mental Simulation? , 2006 .

[12]  Rhiannon N. Turner,et al.  Imagining intergroup contact reduces implicit prejudice. , 2010, The British journal of social psychology.

[13]  D. F. Marks,et al.  Consciousness, mental imagery and action , 1999 .

[14]  Richard T. White,et al.  Recall of autobiographical events , 1989 .

[15]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[16]  James Lynch,et al.  Cultural diversity and the schools , 2006 .

[17]  M. Hewstone,et al.  Direct and Indirect Intergroup Friendship Effects: Testing the Moderating Role of the Affective-Cognitive Bases of Prejudice , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[18]  Richard J. Crisp,et al.  Imagining Intergroup Contact Can Improve Intergroup Attitudes , 2007 .

[19]  Rhiannon N. Turner,et al.  Can imagined interactions produce positive perceptions? Reducing prejudice through simulated social contact. , 2009, The American psychologist.

[20]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Intergroup Contact and Prejudice Toward Immigrants in Italy: The Mediational Role of Anxiety and the Moderational Role of Group Salience , 2003 .

[21]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  The Effect of Mental Simulation on Goal-Directed Performance , 1999 .

[22]  D. Abrams,et al.  Improving intergroup attitudes and reducing stereotype threat: An integrated contact model , 2008 .

[23]  Stephen M. Garcia,et al.  Crowded Minds : The Implicit Bystander Effect , 2002 .

[24]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  Toward a process model of the attitude-behavior relation: Accessing one's attitude upon mere observation of the attitude object. , 1983 .

[25]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior , 1980 .

[26]  Stefania Paolini,et al.  Effects of Direct and Indirect Cross-Group Friendships on Judgments of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland: The Mediating Role of an Anxiety-Reduction Mechanism , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[27]  Mark R. Lepper,et al.  Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  A. Tversky,et al.  The simulation heuristic , 1982 .

[29]  S. McKelvie The VVIQ and beyond: Vividness and its measurement. , 1995 .

[30]  Rhiannon N. Turner,et al.  Have Confidence in Contact. , 2010 .

[31]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[32]  R. Abelson Psychological status of the script concept. , 1981 .

[33]  M. Lynn,et al.  Effects of pictures on the organization and recall of social information. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Harnessing the imagination. Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping. , 1998, The American psychologist.

[35]  A. Bandura Moral Disengagement in the Perpetration of Inhumanities , 1999, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[36]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Self-Relevant Scenarios as Mediators of Likelihood Estimates and Compliance: Does Imagining Make It So? , 1982 .

[37]  Tobias Greitemeyer,et al.  Mental Simulation and the Achievement of Health Goals: The Role of Goal Difficulty , 2006 .

[38]  Donald F. Dansereau,et al.  Effects of Directed Thinking on Intentions to Engage in Beneficial Activities: Actions Versus Reasons1 , 1999 .

[39]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Effects of Script Availability on Social Behavior , 1982 .

[40]  Rhiannon N. Turner,et al.  Reducing prejudice via direct and extended cross-group friendship , 2007 .

[41]  T. Pettigrew Intergroup contact theory. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[42]  Rupert Brown,et al.  An integrative theory of intergroup contact , 2005 .

[43]  D. Landers,et al.  The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis. , 1983 .

[44]  B. Latané,et al.  Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  Rhiannon N. Turner,et al.  Imagined Intergroup Contact: Theory, Paradigm and Practice , 2009 .

[46]  Linda R. Tropp,et al.  A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  Stephen C. Wright,et al.  The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. , 1997 .

[48]  R. Crisp,et al.  Imagining intergroup contact promotes projection to outgroups , 2008 .

[49]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: The mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  S. Cook,et al.  8 – Cooperative Interaction in Multiethnic Contexts* , 1984 .

[51]  D. Abrams,et al.  Threat inoculation: experienced and imagined intergenerational contact prevents stereotype threat effects on older people's math performance. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[52]  Craig A. Anderson,et al.  Decreasing premature termination from psychotherapy , 1987 .

[53]  G. Āllport The Nature of Prejudice , 1954 .

[54]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  From Thought to Action: Effects of Process-Versus Outcome-Based Mental Simulations on Performance , 1999 .