Neural Integration of Risk and Effort Costs by the Frontal Pole: Only upon Request

Rewards in real life are rarely received without incurring costs and successful reward harvesting often involves weighing and minimizing different types of costs. In the natural environment, such costs often include the physical effort required to obtain rewards and potential risks attached to them. Costs may also include potential risks. In this study, we applied fMRI to explore the neural coding of physical effort costs as opposed to costs associated with risky rewards. Using an incentive-compatible valuation mechanism, we separately measured the subjective costs associated with effortful and risky options. As expected, subjective costs of options increased with both increasing effort and increasing risk. Despite the similar nature of behavioral discounting of effort and risk, distinct regions of the brain coded these two cost types separately, with anterior insula primarily processing risk costs and midcingulate and supplementary motor area (SMA) processing effort costs. To investigate integration of the two cost types, we also presented participants with options that combined effortful and risky elements. We found that the frontal pole integrates effort and risk costs through functional coupling with the SMA and insula. The degree to which the latter two regions influenced frontal pole activity correlated with participant-specific behavioral sensitivity to effort and risk costs. These data support the notion that, although physical effort costs may appear to be behaviorally similar to other types of costs, such as risk, they are treated separately at the neural level and are integrated only if there is a need to do so.

[1]  F. B. Vernadat,et al.  Decisions with Multiple Objectives: Preferences and Value Tradeoffs , 1994 .

[2]  R. L. Keeney,et al.  Decisions with Multiple Objectives: Preferences and Value Trade-Offs , 1977, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[3]  John K. Horowitz,et al.  The Becker-DeGroot-Marschak mechanism is not necessarily incentive compatible, even for non-random goods , 2006 .

[4]  H. de Wit,et al.  Determination of discount functions in rats with an adjusting-amount procedure. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[5]  Mathias Pessiglione,et al.  Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  How Green Is the Grass on the Other Side? Frontopolar Cortex and the Evidence in Favor of Alternative Courses of Action , 2009, Neuron.

[7]  Corianne Rogalsky,et al.  Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism , 2003, NeuroImage.

[8]  Peter N. C. Mohr,et al.  Neural Processing of Risk , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Walton,et al.  Comparing the role of the anterior cingulate cortex and 6‐hydroxydopamine nucleus accumbens lesions on operant effort‐based decision making , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[11]  Julian J. Emmanuel,et al.  Metabolic State Alters Economic Decision Making under Risk in Humans , 2010, PloS one.

[12]  J. D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  George I. Christopoulos,et al.  Neural Correlates of Value, Risk, and Risk Aversion Contributing to Decision Making under Risk , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  O. Huber,et al.  Time pressure in risky decision-making: effect on risk defusing , 2007 .

[15]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Weighing up the benefits of work: Behavioral and neural analyses of effort-related decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.

[16]  N. Chater,et al.  Choosing to Make an Effort: The Role of Striatum in Signaling Physical Effort of a Chosen Action , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Soyoung Q. Park,et al.  Neurobiology of Value Integration: When Value Impacts Valuation , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  M. Pessiglione,et al.  Why Don't You Try Harder? An Investigation of Effort Production in Major Depression , 2011, PloS one.

[19]  M. Walton,et al.  Separate neural pathways process different decision costs , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  P. Tobler,et al.  Reward skewness coding in the insula independent of probability and loss. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Jonathan D. Wallis,et al.  Neurons in the Frontal Lobe Encode the Value of Multiple Decision Variables , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  G. Collier,et al.  Operant running as a function of deprivation and effort. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[23]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Effort-Based Cost–Benefit Valuation and the Human Brain , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  D. Zald,et al.  Dopaminergic Mechanisms of Individual Differences in Human Effort-Based Decision-Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  N. Daw,et al.  Human Reinforcement Learning Subdivides Structured Action Spaces by Learning Effector-Specific Values , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Kalyanmoy Deb,et al.  Multiple Criteria Decision Making, Multiattribute Utility Theory: Recent Accomplishments and What Lies Ahead , 2008, Manag. Sci..

[28]  S. Lehéricy,et al.  3-D diffusion tensor axonal tracking shows distinct SMA and pre-SMA projections to the human striatum. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  J. Jacobs,et al.  Regional dendritic and spine variation in human cerebral cortex: a quantitative golgi study. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  S. Quartz,et al.  Human Insula Activation Reflects Risk Prediction Errors As Well As Risk , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Ulrik R Beierholm,et al.  The human prefrontal cortex mediates integration of potential causes behind observed outcomes , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  George I. Christopoulos,et al.  Risk-dependent reward value signal in human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Brain Translates Money into Force: A Neuroimaging Study of Subliminal Motivation , 2007, Science.

[34]  J Tanji,et al.  Overlapping corticostriatal projections from the supplementary motor area and the primary motor cortex in the macaque monkey: An anterograde double labeling study , 1996, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[35]  S. Floresco,et al.  Dopaminergic and Glutamatergic Regulation of Effort- and Delay-Based Decision Making , 2008, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[36]  S. Floresco,et al.  Prefrontal cortical contribution to risk-based decision making. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  J. Lindén,et al.  Eliciting Reservation Prices: Becker–Degroot–Marschak Mechanisms vs. Markets , 1997 .