Differences in effective connectivity between dyslexic children and normal readers during a pseudoword reading task: An fMRI study

PURPOSE This fMRI study investigated phonological and lexicosemantic processing in dyslexic and in chronological age- and reading level-matched children in a pseudoword reading task. MATERIALS AND METHODS The effective connectivity network was compared between the three groups using a structural model including the supramarginal cortex (BA 40; BA: Brodmann area), fusiform cortex (BA 37) and inferior frontal cortex (BA 44/45) areas of the left hemisphere. RESULTS The results revealed differences in connectivity patterns. In dyslexic patients, in contrast with chronological age- and reading level-matched groups, no causal relationship was demonstrated between BA 40 and BA 44/45. However, a significant causal relationship was demonstrated between BA 37 and BA 44/45 both in dyslexic children and in the reading level-matched group. CONCLUSIONS These findings were interpreted as evidence for a phonological deficit in developmental dyslexia.

[1]  J. Erskine,et al.  Proximal Analysis of Developmental Dyslexia in Adulthood: The Cognitive Mosaic Model , 2005 .

[2]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[3]  D. Wegener,et al.  The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  J. Fiez Phonology, Semantics, and the Role of the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex , 2022 .

[5]  S. Petersen,et al.  A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Varieties of developmental dyslexia , 1993, Cognition.

[7]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Segregating Semantic from Phonological Processes during Reading , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study , 2001, Neuroreport.

[9]  W. Kaiser,et al.  fMRI during word processing in dyslexic and normal reading children. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[10]  A. Mechelli,et al.  Reading and reading disturbance , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[11]  Michelle Hampson,et al.  Connectivity–behavior analysis reveals that functional connectivity between left BA39 and Broca's area varies with reading ability , 2006, NeuroImage.

[12]  G Jobard,et al.  Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[13]  Paolo Vitali,et al.  Generating animal and tool names: An fMRI study of effective connectivity , 2005, Brain and Language.

[14]  B. Horwitz,et al.  Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. , 1994 .

[16]  J. Everatt,et al.  Surface and phonological subtypes of adult developmental dyslexia. , 2002, Dyslexia.

[17]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Brain mechanisms for reading: the role of the superior temporal gyrus in word and pseudoword naming , 2000, Neuroreport.

[18]  J. Rumsey,et al.  Failure to activate the left temporoparietal cortex in dyslexia. An oxygen 15 positron emission tomographic study. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[19]  M. Corballis,et al.  Mixed Lateralization of Phonological Assembly in Developmental Dyslexia , 2002, Neurocase.

[20]  M. Habib,et al.  The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: an overview and working hypothesis. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Robert C. MacCallum,et al.  SPECIFICATION SEARCHES IN COVARIANCE STRUCTURE MODELING , 1986 .

[22]  Serge Ruff,et al.  Neural substrates of impaired categorical perception of phonemes in adult dyslexics: An fMRI study , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[23]  Sven Joubert,et al.  Neural correlates of lexical and sublexical processes in reading , 2004, Brain and Language.

[24]  M. Mintun,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during word and nonword reading , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[25]  C. Price The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[26]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[27]  Derek K. Jones,et al.  Perisylvian language networks of the human brain , 2005, Annals of neurology.

[28]  S C Cramer,et al.  Instructional treatment associated with changes in brain activation in children with dyslexia , 2003, Neurology.

[29]  M. Browne,et al.  Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit , 1992 .

[30]  Joseph T Devlin,et al.  The myth of the visual word form area , 2003, NeuroImage.

[31]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[32]  W. Nagy,et al.  Individual fMRI activation in orthographic mapping and morpheme mapping after orthographic or morphological spelling treatment in child dyslexics , 2006, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[33]  P. Bentler,et al.  Significance Tests and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance Structures , 1980 .

[34]  P. Bentler,et al.  Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives , 1999 .

[35]  R. Todd Constable,et al.  Localization of semantic processing using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1994 .

[36]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Bernard Mazoyer,et al.  Word and non-word reading: What role for the Visual Word Form Area? , 2005, NeuroImage.

[39]  E. Bullmore,et al.  How Good Is Good Enough in Path Analysis of fMRI Data? , 2000, NeuroImage.

[40]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures , 1996, Nature.

[41]  F. Ramus Neurobiology of dyslexia: a reinterpretation of the data. , 2004, Trends in neurosciences.

[42]  S. L. Small Different neural circuits subserve reading before and after therapy for acquired dyslexia , 1998 .

[43]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  Jean-François Démonet,et al.  Selective response to letter categorization within the left fusiform gyrus , 2005, NeuroImage.

[45]  Eric Lambert,et al.  NOVLEX : une base de donnes lexicales pour les lves de primaire , 2001 .