Response Inhibition Impairment in High Functioning Autism and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Evidence from Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Data

Background Response inhibition, an important domain of executive function (EF), involves the ability to suppress irrelevant or interfering information and impulses. Previous studies have shown impairment of response inhibition in high functioning autism (HFA) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but more recent findings have been inconsistent. To date, almost no studies have been conducted using functional imaging techniques to directly compare inhibitory control between children with HFA and those with ADHD. Method Nineteen children with HFA, 16 age- and intelligence quotient (IQ)-matched children with ADHD, and 16 typically developing (TD) children were imaged using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) while performing Go/No-go and Stroop tasks. Results Compared with the TD group, children in both the HFA and ADHD groups took more time to respond during the No-go blocks, with reaction time longest for HFA and shortest for TD. Children in the HFA and ADHD groups also made a greater number of reaction errors in the No-go blocks than those in the TD group. During the Stroop task, there were no significant differences between these three groups in reaction time and omission errors. Both the HFA and ADHD groups showed a higher level of inactivation in the right prefrontal cortex (PFC) during the No-go blocks, relative to the TD group. However, no significant differences were found between groups in the levels of oxyhemoglobin concentration in the PFC during the Stroop task. Conclusion Functional brain imaging using NIRS showed reduced activation in the right PFC in children with HFA or ADHD during an inhibition task, indicating that inhibitory dysfunction is a shared feature of both HFA and ADHD.

[1]  Daniel D. Holt,et al.  Inhibitory Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[2]  Frithjof Kruggel,et al.  Near‐infrared spectroscopy can detect brain activity during a color–word matching Stroop task in an event‐related design , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Jan K. Buitelaar,et al.  Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder , 2010, European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry.

[4]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Evidence for a mechanistic model of cognitive control , 2001, Clinical Neuroscience Research.

[5]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  M. Semrud-Clikeman,et al.  Response inhibition and academic abilities in typically developing children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder-combined subtype. , 2010, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[7]  Brian Toone,et al.  Task-specific hypoactivation in prefrontal and temporoparietal brain regions during motor inhibition and task switching in medication-naive children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  J. Russell,et al.  The execution of arbitrary procedures by children with autism , 2001, Development and Psychopathology.

[9]  Herbert Roeyers,et al.  The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response variability in child psychopathology , 2006, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[10]  L. Saksida,et al.  Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak central coherence effects: experiments in visual configural learning and auditory perception. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approaches toward a model of cognitive control. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[12]  Matthias L. Schroeter,et al.  Prefrontal brain activation during stop-signal response inhibition: An event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  D. Bishop,et al.  Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology , 2005, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[14]  C. Jarrold,et al.  Inhibition and the Validity of the Stroop Task for Children with Autism , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[15]  D. Murphy,et al.  Neural Correlates of Executive Function in Autistic Spectrum Disorders , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[16]  Larry J. Seidman,et al.  Functional MRI in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for hypofrontality , 2005, Brain and Development.

[17]  Sally Ozonoff,et al.  Examining the Relationship between Executive Functions and Restricted, Repetitive Symptoms of Autistic Disorder , 2005, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[18]  Tor D. Wager,et al.  Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[19]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[20]  Q. Luo,et al.  Gender difference in hemodynamic responses of prefrontal area to emotional stress by near-infrared spectroscopy , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[21]  J. R. Wiersema,et al.  HFA and ADHD: A direct comparison on state regulation and response inhibition , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[22]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  Herbert Roeyers,et al.  How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[24]  Vincent Giampietro,et al.  Dissociated functional brain abnormalities of inhibition in boys with pure conduct disorder and in boys with pure attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2008, The American journal of psychiatry.

[25]  B. Astor,et al.  Subtle Executive Impairment in Children with Autism and Children with ADHD , 2005, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[26]  S. Ozonoff,et al.  Brief Report: Specific Executive Function Profiles in Three Neurodevelopmental Disorders , 1999, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[27]  B. Dritschel,et al.  Executive functions in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[28]  Katya Rubia,et al.  Disorder‐specific dysfunction in right inferior prefrontal cortex during two inhibition tasks in boys with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder compared to boys with obsessive–compulsive disorder , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[29]  Matthew W. Mosconi,et al.  Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders , 2009, Psychological Medicine.

[30]  Ann-Christine Ehlis,et al.  Optical topography during a Go–NoGo task assessed with multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  C. Jarrold,et al.  The Problem with Using Eye-Gaze to Infer Desire: A Deficit of Cue Inference in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder? , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[32]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Inhibitory Function in Nonretarded Children with Autism , 1997, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[33]  M. Just,et al.  From the Selectedworks of Marcel Adam Just Inhibitory Control in High Functioning Autism: Decreased Activation and Underconnectivity in Inhibition Networks Inhibitory Control in High-functioning Autism: Decreased Activation and Underconnectivity in Inhibition Networks , 2022 .

[34]  Jacob A. Burack,et al.  Intact Covert Orienting to Peripheral Cues Among Children with Autism , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[35]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.