Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations

Attaching meaning to arbitrary symbols (i.e. words) is a complex and lengthy process. In the case of numbers, it was previously suggested that this process is grounded on two early pre-verbal systems for numerical quantification: the approximate number system (ANS or 'analogue magnitude'), and the object tracking system (OTS or 'parallel individuation'), which children are equipped with before symbolic learning. Each system is based on dedicated neural circuits, characterized by specific computational limits, and each undergoes a separate developmental trajectory. Here, I review the available cognitive and neuroscientific data and argue that the available evidence is more consistent with a crucial role for the ANS, rather than for the OTS, in the acquisition of abstract numerical concepts that are uniquely human.

[1]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[2]  V. Michel,et al.  Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic , 2009, Science.

[3]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction , 2007, Nature.

[4]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  The Neural Development of an Abstract Concept of Number , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Chun,et al.  Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects , 2006, Nature.

[6]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[8]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. , 2008, Developmental science.

[9]  Fei Xu,et al.  Linking Visual Attention and Number Processing in the Brain: The Role of the Temporo-parietal Junction in Small and Large Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Number Comparison , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[11]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[12]  Susan Carey,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Arithmetic: Evolution and Ontogenisis , 2001 .

[13]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses , 2008, Cognition.

[14]  Daniel C. Hyde,et al.  All Numbers Are Not Equal: An Electrophysiological Investigation of Small and Large Number Representations , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Dehaene Origins of Mathematical Intuitions , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  David C Burr,et al.  Temporal auditory capture does not affect the time course of saccadic mislocalization of visual stimuli. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[17]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[18]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Calibrating the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[19]  C. Packer,et al.  Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  Bertrand Thirion,et al.  Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns , 2009, Current Biology.

[21]  Wim Fias,et al.  Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  S. Luck,et al.  The development of visual short-term memory capacity in infants. , 2003, Child development.

[23]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Semantic priming in number naming , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[24]  Z. Pylyshyn Visual indexes, preconceptual objects, and situated vision , 2001, Cognition.

[25]  David C Burr,et al.  Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[26]  Sara Cordes,et al.  Crossing the divide: infants discriminate small from large numerosities. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[27]  R. Johnston,et al.  Exploring the roles of the visual‐spatial sketch pad and central executive in children's arithmetical skills: Views from cognition and developmental neuropsychology , 1999 .

[28]  D G Gadian,et al.  Calculation difficulties in children of very low birthweight: a neural correlate. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  How Humans Count: Numerosity and the Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[30]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Basic Math in Monkeys and College Students , 2007, PLoS biology.

[31]  W. Meck Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2003 .

[32]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  A Computational Model of Number Comparison , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[33]  H. Coslett,et al.  Simultanagnosia. To see but not two see. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[35]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[36]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994 .

[37]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional and Structural Alterations of the Intraparietal Sulcus in a Developmental Dyscalculia of Genetic Origin , 2003, Neuron.

[38]  Jesse Snedeker,et al.  When is Four Far More Than Three? Children’s Generalization of Newly-Acquired Number Words , 2009 .

[39]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[40]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers , 2005, Cognition.

[42]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[43]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. , 2010, Developmental science.

[44]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Children's understanding of counting , 1990, Cognition.

[45]  Michael Andres,et al.  Mode-dependent and mode-independent representations of numerosity in the right intraparietal sulcus , 2010, NeuroImage.

[46]  G J Hitch,et al.  Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  S. A. Rose,et al.  Visual short-term memory in the first year of life: capacity and recency effects. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[48]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[49]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Monkeys Match the Number of Voices They Hear to the Number of Faces They See , 2005, Current Biology.

[50]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[51]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.

[52]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults , 2005, Neuroreport.

[53]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[54]  M. Posner,et al.  Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  J. Cantlon,et al.  Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.

[56]  P. Bryant,et al.  Categorizing sounds and learning to read—a causal connection , 1983, Nature.

[57]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mathematics anxiety affects counting but not subitizing during visual enumeration , 2010, Cognition.

[58]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Subitizing: The preverbal counting process. , 1991 .

[59]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Spontaneous analog number representations in 3-year-old children. , 2010, Developmental science.

[60]  D. Ansari Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[61]  G. Fechner Elemente der Psychophysik , 1998 .

[62]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[63]  Bahador Bahrami,et al.  A Candidate for the Attentional Bottleneck: Set-size Specific Modulation of the Right TPJ during Attentive Enumeration , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex , 2004 .

[65]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The magnitude representation of small and large symbolic numbers in the left and right hemisphere: An event-related fMRI study , 2010 .

[66]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Infants’ auditory enumeration: Evidence for analog magnitudes in the small number range , 2009, Cognition.

[67]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[68]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Science.

[69]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Single-trial classification of parallel pre-attentive and serial attentive processes using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[70]  Bruce E. Lyon,et al.  Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism , 2003, Nature.

[71]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[72]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students , 2004, Cognition.

[73]  T. Verguts,et al.  Dissecting the symbolic distance effect: Comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[74]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  The Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects , 2004, Psychological science.

[75]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Age-related Changes in the Activation of the Intraparietal Sulcus during Nonsymbolic Magnitude Processing: An Event-related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[77]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Semantic Associations between Signs and Numerical Categories in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, PLoS biology.

[78]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Nonverbal representations of time and number in animals and human infants. , 2003 .

[79]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Rapid Development of Feature Binding in Visual Short-Term Memory , 2006, Psychological science.

[80]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[81]  K. Wynn Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[82]  B. Scholl Objects and attention: the state of the art , 2001, Cognition.

[83]  J. Jay Todd,et al.  Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex , 2004, Nature.

[84]  J. Krebs,et al.  An introduction to behavioural ecology , 1981 .

[85]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[86]  R. Shalev,et al.  Developmental Dyscalculia , 2004, Journal of child neurology.

[87]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.

[88]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants , 2008, PLoS biology.

[89]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[90]  S. Vecera,et al.  Psychoanatomical substrates of Bálint's syndrome , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[91]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[92]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Does Subitizing Reflect Numerical Estimation? , 2022 .

[93]  George A. Alvarez,et al.  The capacity of visual short-term memory is set by total information load, not number of objects , 2010 .

[94]  Gavin R. Price,et al.  Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia , 2007, Current Biology.

[95]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Children's representation of symbolic magnitude: the development of the priming distance effect. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[96]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[97]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[98]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Dynamic population coding of category information in inferior temporal and prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[99]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[100]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[101]  S. Dehaene Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .

[102]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? , 2004, Cognition.

[103]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Neural Evidence Linking Visual Object Enumeration and Attention , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[104]  N. R. Franks,et al.  Chimpanzees and the mathematics of battle , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.