Stereotype threat in classroom settings: the interactive effect of domain identification, task difficulty and stereotype threat on female students' maths performance.

BACKGROUND Stereotype threat research revealed that negative stereotypes can disrupt the performance of persons targeted by such stereotypes. This paper contributes to stereotype threat research by providing evidence that domain identification and the difficulty level of test items moderate stereotype threat effects on female students' maths performance. AIMS The study was designed to test theoretical ideas derived from stereotype threat theory and assumptions outlined in the Yerkes-Dodson law proposing a nonlinear relationship between arousal, task difficulty and performance. SAMPLE Participants were 108 high school students attending secondary schools. METHOD Participants worked on a test comprising maths problems of different difficulty levels. Half of the participants learned that the test had been shown to produce gender differences (stereotype threat). The other half learned that the test had been shown not to produce gender differences (no threat). The degree to which participants identify with the domain of maths was included as a quasi-experimental factor. RESULTS Maths-identified female students showed performance decrements under conditions of stereotype threat. Moreover, the stereotype threat manipulation had different effects on low and high domain identifiers' performance depending on test item difficulty. On difficult items, low identifiers showed higher performance under threat (vs. no threat) whereas the reverse was true in high identifiers. This interaction effect did not emerge on easy items. CONCLUSIONS Domain identification and test item difficulty are two important factors that need to be considered in the attempt to understand the impact of stereotype threat on performance.

[1]  T. Claire,et al.  Extending the Concept of Stereotype Threat to Social Class: The Intellectual Underperformance of Students from Low Socioeconomic Backgrounds , 1998 .

[2]  A. Whaley Issues of validity in empirical tests of stereotype threat theory. , 1998 .

[3]  P. Peterson,et al.  2 – Autonomous Learning Behavior: A Possible Explanation of Gender-Related Differences in Mathematics , 1985 .

[4]  John M. Darley,et al.  Stereotype Threat Effects on Black and White Athletic Performance , 1999 .

[5]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging. , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[6]  E. Fennema,et al.  Gender differences in mathematics performance: a meta-analysis. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  C. Steele A threat in the air. How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[8]  Michael Johns,et al.  Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  M. Zanna,et al.  Establishing a causal chain: why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Michael Johns,et al.  Knowing Is Half the Battle , 2005, Psychological science.

[11]  Paul H. White,et al.  Development of the Domain Identification Measure: A Tool for Investigating Stereotype Threat Effects , 2001 .

[12]  Olejnik,et al.  Measures of Effect Size for Comparative Studies: Applications, Interpretations, and Limitations. , 2000, Contemporary educational psychology.

[13]  C. Stangor,et al.  Activating stereotypes undermines task performance expectations , 1998 .

[14]  Jim Blascovich,et al.  African Americans and High Blood Pressure: The Role of Stereotype Threat , 2001, Psychological science.

[15]  Catherine Good,et al.  When White Men Can't Do Math: Necessary and Sufficient Factors in Stereotype Threat , 1999 .

[16]  P. Huguet,et al.  Stereotype Threat Undermines Intellectual Performance by Triggering a Disruptive Mental Load , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[17]  R D Bock,et al.  Further evidence of sex-linked major-gene influence on human spatial visualizing ability. , 1973, American journal of human genetics.

[18]  Christian S. Crandall,et al.  Stereotype Threat and Arousal: Effects on Women's Math Performance , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[19]  H. Markus,et al.  The effect of mere presence on social facilitation: An unobtrusive test , 1978 .

[20]  Jennifer K. Bosson,et al.  When saying and doing diverge: The effects ofstereotype threat on self-reported versus nonverbal anxiety , 2004 .

[21]  R. Yerkes,et al.  The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation , 1908 .

[22]  Margaret Walsh,et al.  Influence of Item Content and Stereotype Situation on Gender Differences in Mathematical Problem Solving , 1999 .

[23]  L. V. Jones White-Black Achievement Differences: The Narrowing Gap. , 1984 .

[24]  S. Spencer,et al.  Stereotype Threat and Women's Math Performance , 1999 .

[25]  Johannes Keller,et al.  Stereotype Threat in the Classroom: Dejection Mediates the Disrupting Threat Effect on Women’s Math Performance , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[26]  Jill E. Ellingson,et al.  High-stakes testing in employment, credentialing, and higher education. Prospects in a post-affirmative-action world. , 2001, The American psychologist.

[27]  E. E. Jones,et al.  Control of Attributions about the Self Through Self-handicapping Strategies: The Appeal of Alcohol and the Role of Underachievement , 1978 .

[28]  C. Steele,et al.  Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  Robert B. Zajonc,et al.  Social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses , 1966 .

[30]  Toni Schmader,et al.  Gender Identification Moderates Stereotype Threat Effects on Women's Math Performance ☆ ☆☆ ★ , 2002 .

[31]  Joseph M. Hillery,et al.  Social Facilitation in a Coaction Setting: An Examination of the Effects Over Learning Trials. , 1973 .

[32]  Jerre Levy Cerebral lateralization and spatial ability , 1976, Behavior genetics.

[33]  N. Ambady,et al.  Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity Salience and Shifts in Quantitative Performance , 1999 .

[34]  Anne Maass,et al.  Stereotype threat: The effect of expectancy on performance. , 2003 .

[35]  R. Sorrentino,et al.  Achievement, affiliation, and group incentives: A test of the overmotivation hypothesis , 1986 .

[36]  Denise Sekaquaptewa,et al.  Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance , 2003 .

[37]  Katherine E. Ryan,et al.  Psychological Processes Underlying Stereotype Threat and Standardized Math Test Performance , 2005 .

[38]  Johannes Keller,et al.  Blatant Stereotype Threat and Women's Math Performance: Self-Handicapping as a Strategic Means to Cope with Obtrusive Negative Performance Expectations , 2002 .

[39]  J. Croizet,et al.  Stereotype Threat: Are Lower Status and History of Stigmatization Preconditions of Stereotype Threat? , 2000 .

[40]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  Arousal and stereotype threat , 2005 .