Walking the Hedonic Product Treadmill: Default Contrast and Mood-Based Assimilation in Judgments of Predicted Happiness with a Target Product
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Duane T. Wegener,et al. Flexible Correction Processes in Social Judgment: Correcting for Context-Induced Contrast , 1993 .
[2] Mengze Shi,et al. Virtual Progress: The Effect of Path Characteristics on Perceptions of Progress and Choice , 2003, Manag. Sci..
[3] H. Helson,et al. Adaptation-level theory , 1964 .
[4] John G. Lynch,et al. Contrast Effects in Consumer Judgments: Changes in Mental Representations or in the Anchoring of Rating Scales? , 1991 .
[5] Paco Underhill,et al. Why We Buy: The Science of Shopping , 1999 .
[6] L. L. Martin,et al. Set/reset: use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[7] Pham,et al. Comparison Opportunity and Judgment Revision. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.
[8] G. Bower. Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.
[9] G. McClelland,et al. Misleading Heuristics and Moderated Multiple Regression Models , 2001 .
[10] Daniel Kahneman,et al. Experiences extended across time: Evaluation of moments and episodes , 1992 .
[11] J. Edell,et al. The Power of Feelings in Understanding Advertising Effects , 1987 .
[12] H. Marsh,et al. Natural anchoring at the neutral point of category rating scales , 1978 .
[13] Russell S. Winer,et al. A reference price model of brand choice for frequently purchased products. , 1986 .
[14] M. Zeelenberg,et al. The impact of accuracy motivation on interpretation, comparison, and correction processes: accuracy x knowledge accessibility effects. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[15] D A Stapel,et al. Let’s Not Forget the Past When We Go to the Future: On Our Knowledge of Knowledge Accessibility , 2001 .
[16] G. Clore,et al. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. , 1983 .
[17] D. Ariely. Combining experiences over time: the effects of duration, intensity changes and on‐line measurements on retrospective pain evaluations , 1998 .
[18] Barbara A. Mellers,et al. Equity judgment: A revision of Aristotelian views. , 1982 .
[19] Dale T. Miller,et al. Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .
[20] Norbert Schwarz,et al. Constructing reality and its alternatives: an inclusion/ exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment , 1992 .
[21] H. Simon,et al. A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice , 1955 .
[22] D. Campbell,et al. Hedonic relativism and planning the good society , 1971 .
[23] M. Holbrook,et al. Assessing the Role of Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Advertising , 1987 .
[24] P. Herr,et al. On the consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects , 1983 .
[25] P. Brickman,et al. Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative? , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[26] Michel Tuan Pham. Representativeness, Relevance, and the Use of Feelings in Decision Making , 1998 .
[27] Dan Padgett,et al. Patterns of Affective Reactions to Advertisements: The Integration of Moment-to-Moment Responses into Overall Judgments , 1997 .
[28] Karen A. Machleit,et al. A Comparison of Attitudes and Emotions as Predictors of Behavior at Diverse Levels of Behavioral Experience , 1992 .
[29] P. Herr. Priming Price: Prior Knowledge and Context Effects , 1989 .
[30] D. Kahneman,et al. When More Pain Is Preferred to Less: Adding a Better End , 1993 .
[31] Michael H. Birnbaum,et al. Contextual effects in social judgment , 1983 .
[32] Barbara Bickart,et al. Carryover and Backfire Effects in Marketing Research , 1993 .
[33] M. Goldberg,et al. Mood, Awareness, and Product Evaluation , 1993 .
[34] Patrick S. Malone,et al. Unbelieving the Unbelievable: Some problems in the rejection of false information , 1990 .
[35] S. West,et al. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. , 1994 .
[36] Dan Ariely,et al. On the making of an experience: the effects of breaking and combining experiences on their overall evaluation , 2000 .
[37] H. Helson. Adaptation-level as frame of reference for prediction of psychophysical data. , 1947, The American journal of psychology.
[38] Brian Sternthal,et al. A Two-Factor Explanation of Assimilation and Contrast Effects , 1993 .
[39] J. Elster,et al. Choice Over Time , 1992 .
[40] D. Kahneman,et al. Patients' memories of painful medical treatments: real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures , 1996, Pain.
[41] Mark I. Alpert,et al. Music influences on mood and purchase intentions , 1990 .
[42] Drazen Prelec,et al. Preferences for sequences of outcomes. , 1993 .
[43] A Parducci,et al. The category effect in social judgment: experimental ratings of happiness. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[44] D. Stapel,et al. Using primed exemplars during impression formation: Interpretation or comparison? , 1997 .
[45] Alice M. Tybout,et al. Context Effects at Encoding and Judgment in Consumption Settings: The Role of Cognitive Resources , 1997 .
[46] Robert P. Abelson,et al. Velocity relation : satisfaction as a function of the first derivative of outcome over time , 1991 .
[47] B. Kahn,et al. Cross-category effects of induced arousal and pleasure on the Internet shopping experience , 2002 .
[48] D. Ariely,et al. When does duration matter in judgment and decision making? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[49] A. Isen,et al. Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: a cognitive loop? , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.