The Force of Numbers: Investigating Manual Signatures of Embodied Number Processing
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Olivier Renaud,et al. Permutation Tests for Regression, ANOVA, and Comparison of Signals: The permuco Package , 2021, J. Stat. Softw..
[2] M. H. Fischer,et al. Stimulating numbers: signatures of finger counting in numerosity processing , 2020, Psychological research.
[3] Karima Mersad,et al. Time dependency of the SNARC effect for different number formats: evidence from saccadic responses , 2018, Psychological Research.
[4] Thomas J. Faulkenberry,et al. Tracking the Continuous Dynamics of Numerical Processing: A Brief Review and Editorial , 2018, J. Numer. Cogn..
[5] E. Hubbard,et al. On the genesis of spatial-numerical associations: Evolutionary and cultural factors co-construct the mental number line , 2018, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[6] Y. Delevoye-Turrell,et al. Seeing action simulation as it unfolds: The implicit effects of action scenes on muscle contraction evidenced through the use of a grip-force sensor , 2018, Neuropsychologia.
[7] M. H. Fischer,et al. Deconstructing spatial-numerical associations , 2018, Cognition.
[8] M. H. Fischer,et al. Commentary: The mental representation of integers: An abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts , 2018, Front. Psychol..
[9] V. Frak,et al. Manual action verbs modulate the grip force of each hand in unimanual or symmetrical bimanual tasks , 2018, PloS one.
[10] David C. Geary,et al. Language and culture in mathematical cognition , 2018 .
[11] Michael J. Wiemers,et al. Two Attributes of Number Meaning: Numerical Associations With Visual Space and Size Exist in Parallel , 2017, Experimental psychology.
[12] Oliver Lindemann,et al. Finger posing primes number comprehension , 2017, Cognitive Processing.
[13] R. Núñez. Is There Really an Evolved Capacity for Number? , 2017, Trends in cognitive sciences.
[14] Y. Paulignan,et al. A simple technique to study embodied language processes: the grip force sensor , 2017, Behavior research methods.
[15] R. Verleger,et al. Go and no-go P3 with rare and frequent stimuli in oddball tasks: A study comparing key-pressing with counting. , 2016, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[16] Agustín Ibáñez,et al. A touch with words: Dynamic synergies between manual actions and language , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[17] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Gamble on gaze: Eye movements reflect the numerical value of blackjack hands , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[18] F. Mast,et al. Counting is a spatial process: evidence from eye movements , 2016, Psychological research.
[19] A. Cangelosi,et al. Ocular drift along the mental number line , 2016, Psychological Research.
[20] Aditya Jain,et al. A comparative study of visual and auditory reaction times on the basis of gender and physical activity levels of medical first year students , 2015, International journal of applied & basic medical research.
[21] M. H. Fischer,et al. Embodied number processing , 2015 .
[22] Ann Dowker,et al. The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition , 2015 .
[23] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Pushing forward in embodied cognition: may we mouse the mathematical mind? , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[24] Samuel Shaki,et al. Spatial Associations in Numerical Cognition—From Single Digits to Arithmetic , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[25] J. Tzelgov,et al. Grasping numbers: evidence for automatic influence of numerical magnitude on grip aperture , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[26] Oliver Lindemann,et al. Expyriment: A Python library for cognitive and neuroscientific experiments , 2014, Behavior research methods.
[27] Viviane Deprez,et al. Action relevance in linguistic context drives word-induced motor activity , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[28] Ivan Toni,et al. Different Brains Process Numbers Differently: Structural Bases of Individual Differences in Spatial and Nonspatial Number Representations , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[30] Robyn Carson,et al. Easy methods for extracting individual regression slopes: Comparing SPSS, R, and Excel , 2013 .
[31] M. Fabbri. Finger Counting Habits and Spatial-Numerical Association in Horizontal and Vertical Orientations , 2013 .
[32] Viviane Deprez,et al. Grip Force Reveals the Context Sensitivity of Language-Induced Motor Activity during “Action Words” Processing: Evidence from Sentential Negation , 2012, PloS one.
[33] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Meaning and the brain: The neurosemantics of referential, interactive, and combinatorial knowledge , 2012, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[34] Martin H. Fischer,et al. A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition , 2012, Cognitive Processing.
[35] P. Jaśkowski,et al. Are low and high number magnitudes processed differently while resolving the conflict evoked by the SNARC effect? , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[36] Frank Domahs,et al. Finger Counting and Numerical Cognition , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[37] Yi-Fang Hsu,et al. The time course of symbolic number adaptation: Oscillatory EEG activity and event-related potential analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.
[38] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers , 2012, NeuroImage.
[39] Jan Theeuwes,et al. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.
[40] Guilherme Wood,et al. A Hand Full of Numbers: A Role for Offloading in Arithmetics Learning? , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[41] Anna M. Borghi,et al. Graspable Objects Shape Number Processing , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[42] Oliver Lindemann,et al. Finger Counting Habits in Middle Eastern and Western Individuals: An Online Survey , 2011 .
[43] Martin H. Fischer,et al. How to cook a SNARC: Number placement in text rapidly changes spatial–numerical associations , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[44] Marc Jeannerod,et al. Grip Force Is Part of the Semantic Representation of Manual Action Verbs , 2010, PloS one.
[45] A. Kiesel,et al. Congruency effects between number magnitude and response force. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[46] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Understanding in an instant: Neurophysiological evidence for mechanistic language circuits in the brain , 2009, Brain and Language.
[47] Samuel Shaki,et al. Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[48] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic , 2008, Cognition.
[49] Guilherme Wood,et al. On the Cognitive Link between Space and Number: A Meta-Analysis of the SNARC Effect , 2008 .
[50] S. Mitra,et al. Sub-processes of working memory in the N-back task: An investigation using ERPs , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[51] Jeff Miller,et al. Does the semantic activation of quantity representations influence motor parameters? , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] Masami Ishihara,et al. Touch perception reveals the dominance of spatial over digital representation of numbers , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[53] David J. Ostry,et al. Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping , 2008, Cortex.
[54] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations , 2008, Cortex.
[55] Roi Cohen Kadosh,et al. Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental and comparative studies of magnitude representation , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.
[56] L. Barsalou. Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[57] Harold Bekkering,et al. Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[58] R. Oostenveld,et al. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[59] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Spatial associations in number-related tasks: A comparison of manual and pedal responses. , 2006, Experimental psychology.
[60] Xavier Seron,et al. Finger–digit compatibility in Arabic numeral processing , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[61] Wim Fias,et al. Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2006, Experimental psychology.
[62] L. Jonkman,et al. The development of preparation, conflict monitoring and inhibition from early childhood to young adulthood; a Go/Nogo ERP study , 2006, Brain Research.
[63] Wim Fias,et al. Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[64] Michael Andres,et al. Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction , 2004, Neuroreport.
[65] Vincent Walsh. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[66] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Spatial representations in number processing--evidence from a pointing task , 2003 .
[67] M. Schlag-Rey,et al. Through the eye, slowly; Delays and localization errors in the visual system , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[68] S Rossi,et al. Corticospinal excitability modulation to hand muscles during movement imagery. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.
[69] Pierre Barrouillet,et al. Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuro-psychological performance: a longitudinal study , 1998, Cognition.
[70] Peter Brugger,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[71] W. Fias. The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .
[72] A. Gevins,et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[73] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[74] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.
[75] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[76] D. Balota,et al. Automatic and attentional priming in young and older adults: reevaluation of the two-process model. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[77] William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Function and process in spoken word recognition: A tutorial review , 1984 .
[78] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[79] S. S. Stevens,et al. Ratio scales and category scales for a dozen perceptual continua. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.
[80] S. S. Stevens. On the psychophysical law. , 1957, Psychological review.